Detailed guides to painful problems, treatments & more

The Complete Guide to Low Back Pain

An extremely detailed guide to the myths, controversies, and treatment options for low back pain

Paul Ingraham • 900m read
Large banner image of a vintage illustration of the lumbar spine from an oblique perspective, no caption, vaguely representing analysis of low back pain.

We can put a man on the moon, but back pain is just as miserable as ever, and more costly to society than ever. Most information available online is awful.1 There are no miracle cures or back whisperers. Not one popular treatment has ever been shown to work well.2 When we are “shot by the witch,”3 it’s going to run its course like a head cold in most cases, or drag on much longer in a few, and there’s not much anyone can do about it.

There is room for improvement, however! We could, at least, not add insult to injury with pointless and expensive testing, therapies, and surgeries that are all about finding and fixing structural problems that are mostly not there, or not the real problem.

Despite overwhelming scientific evidence to the contrary, it is still sadly routine for back pain to be seen as a “mechanical” problem, as if the spine is a fragile structure which breaks down.4 There is some truth in that old way of looking at it, but there are many other factors in back pain. It’s hard to treat because “it’s complicated.” Like cancer, back pain is not one disease, but a large family of disorders, many of them cryptic.

Has nobody noticed the embarrassing fact that science is about to clone a human being, but it still can’t cure the pain of a bad back?

Pain, by Marni Jackson, p. 5

A tragic low back pain myth

This pervasive myth of spinal fragility has many unfortunate consequences, such as unnecessary fusion surgeries — a common and routinely ineffective procedure — and low back pain that lasts for years instead of months or weeks. The seriousness of chronic low back pain is often emphasized in terms of the hair-raising economic costs of work absenteeism, but it may well be far worse than that — a recent Swedish study shows that it probably even shortens lives.5 The stakes are high. “Tragedy” is not hyperbole.

Even worse is that good information exists: many medical experts do “get it,” but they have fought a long, losing battle trying to spread the word to their own medical colleagues on the front lines of health care.6 Back pain treatment in the real world is notoriously out of step with guidelines and best practices,7 and the average family doctor is just not educated enough.8

And as if that wasn’t bad enough, doctors who are more interested in back pain are even worse,9 probably because a little knowledge is a dangerous thing. Similarly, experts have particularly struggled to get the word to alternative health professionals — most of whom don’t even read medical journals.10

In this tutorial, you will meet those medical experts and find out what they know and believe and why. Their ideas about low back pain are neither “conventional” nor “alternative” — they simply come from the best minds in the business.

The evidence that tissue pathology does not explain chronic pain is overwhelming (e.g., in back pain, neck pain, and knee osteoarthritis).

Lorimer Moseley, “Teaching people about pain — why do we keep beating around the bush?Pain Management. 2012.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

How do so many health care professionals go wrong when they treat low back pain?

Why does the myth of mechanical back pain, the premise for so much ineffective treatment, get repeated endlessly on the Internet and in health care offices around the world? (All of these points above will be explained and substantiated in detail further along — these are just the highlights.)

Plus, of course, there are an almost unbelievable number of sketchier treatments for sale, easily marketed to desperate souls. Is low back pain treatment really this much of a mess? Sadly, I believe so.15 I clearly remember graduating clueless myself. If I hadn’t spent many years doing post-grad study of low back pain, I wouldn’t know 98% of what’s in this tutorial.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Stereotypical stock photo of a guy in a suit meditating, sitting cross-legged in a field with a blue sky background, representing the common belief back pain might be helped by meditating and/or yoga and stress relief in general.

Over-rated?

Yes, stress is one factor in low back pain … but meditation, yoga, relaxation & other mind-body treatments are over-rated & inappropriate for many people. This guide does not deny the role of the mind & explores it thoroughly — but the focus is on more practical options.

The journey to relief begins with better back beliefs

These are the most important back pain “belief upgrades”:

Each of these contradicts a major myth or two about back pain, and is strongly supported by the science.16 By the end of this tutorial, I hope you’re convinced of each one. But initially? Most people will have trouble swallowing them. Even professionals will — especially the ones whose income depends on denying these.

Embracing them, instead of the myth of fragility and all its implications, is half the battle. But it’s really tough when you’re facing intense, chronic back pain. Sure, most acute low back pain fades steadily — up to 90% of it, for uncomplicated cases.17 And so does a lot of so-called “chronic” low back pain!18

But not all. This tutorial is mainly for patients with unusually stubborn low back pain and sciatica, and for the doctors and therapists who want to help. (It’s overkill for new and acute cases.) Even for these really entrenched cases, there is hope…

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Thumbnail image of “Back Facts” infographic about back pain.

An excellent “back facts” infographic from O’Sullivan et al. Examples: getting older is not a cause of back pain, poor posture does not cause back pain & scans rarely show the cause.

Click to embiggen.

The case for hope: some “incurable” chronic low back pain can still be cured

While it’s true that most chronic lower back pain will not yield to any popular back pain treatment, it’s also true that some really stubborn “incurable” cases do eventually turn out to be curable. People who believed for years that their pain was invincible have still found relief. Not always, and often not completely — but sometimes any relief is far better than nothing. How can extremely stubborn pain finally ease up? Simple: because many cases weren’t truly stubborn to begin with, despite all appearances. So many health professionals are poorly prepared to treat low back pain that patients can easily go for months or even years without once getting good care and advice. When they finally get it, it’s hardly surprising that some patients finally get some relief from their pain.

And it’s always amazing to me how chronic pain can, with the right approach, finally melt away — it’s not common, but it does happen. Lots of people who thought they’d “tried everything” for lower back pain read this tutorial and then write to me and say, “Well, I guess I hadn’t tried everything!”

Similarly, many athletes with “career-ending” injuries are far from finished. Bret “The Glute Guy” Contreras, from You’ll Never Squat Again:

Numerous powerlifters over the years have come back following ‘career-ending injuries’ to set all-time personal records. Donnie Thompson is the only man to total 3,000 lbs (1,265 lb squat, 950 lb bench, 785 lb deadlift). Many people don’t know this, but several years back Donnie suffered a horrendous back injury and herniated three discs. He could barely walk, but he got out of bed and rehabbed himself every day. Within three months he was back to heavy squatting and setting personal records. Got that? Setting personal records three months following an injury that herniated 3 discs!

How could that be? It’s almost like herniated discs aren’t necessarily as scary as everyone seems to think. Hm!

I have never met a patient — no matter how experienced or self-educated — who could not gain at least some new insights and new hope from this tutorial.19

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Illustration of a lumbar vertebra, side view, with bold blue labels for the body, spinous process, and three processes: spinous, superior, and inferior.

A lumbar vertebra

Vertebrae are the scaffolding of the back. A lot of back pain doesn’t have much to do with spinal structure, especially the bones themselves, but you do need to know a little about the anatomy to properly wrap your head around the subject.

“What if there’s something seriously wrong in there?”

“How do you know I’m not seriously hurt?”

“Could it be cancer? A tumor?”

You’re not paranoid if they really are after you! Only about 1% of back pain has a nasty cause,20 and only a few of those are really scary. But it happens. Andy Whitfield, star of Spartacus, thought he just had back pain from his intense gladiatorial training. In fact, he had a tumour. It killed him in 2011.

The most dangerous thing about trying to reassure low back pain patients is the unnerving possibility that I might reassure someone who should not be. But reassurance is almost always appropriate. Most back injury feels worse than it is — its bark is worse than its bite.

But how do you know if you’re the exception? Can you recognize the early warning sign of cancer, infection, autoimmune disease, or spinal cord injury? These things often cause other distinctive signs and symptoms, and so they are usually diagnosed promptly. If you are aware of these red flags, you can get checked out when the time is right — but please avoid excessive worry before that.

The rule of thumb21 is that you should start a more thorough medical investigation only when three conditions are met:

  1. it’s been bothering you for more than about six weeks
  2. the trend is strongly negative — the pain is severe and/or not improving, or even getting worse
  3. there is at least one other red flag (see below)

And there are also two rare situations where you shouldn’t wait several weeks before deciding the situation is serious …

  1. Pain and weakness in both legs, especially if it’s also hard to pee.22
  2. Any accident with forces that may have been sufficient to fracture your spine. Please seek thorough medical assessment promptly, including an X-ray to look for a fracture. You really do need an X-ray to ensure that your spine is not actually broken. They aren’t necessarily as obvious as you’d think!

This free article explains in more detail (including a list of red flags) and is strongly recommended to anyone who feels nervous:

In all other cases, you can safely read this tutorial first. For instance, even if you have severe pain or numbness and tingling down your leg, you can safely read this first. Or, even if you have an obviously severe muscle tear from trying to lift your car or something, you can safely start here — rest and read. Your back is not as fragile as you probably think, and understanding why is a great starting place for healing in nearly all cases of low back pain.

Don’t confuse threat and risk. Working at the edge is a risk. But then again, so is walking out your front door.

Cory Blickenstaff, PT

Is back pain a symptom of COVID-19? (Or other common infections?)

Back pain is not specifically a prominent feature of COVID-19. However, infections always lower our pain thresholds, and so all common aches and pains are more likely to be triggered or aggravated by any systemic infection — but perhaps COVID-19 more than most, simply because it’s unusually good at causing widespread body aching,23 and backs are included in that. But back pain doesn’t stand out any more than any other common locations for aching or soreness (with the exception of headache, which occurs in 8–14% of cases.2425).

So, if you were already at risk of a flare-up of back pain, it could emerge during any infection, exposed like a rock that is only visible at low tide.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Part 2

Low Back Pain Diagnosis & The Nature of the Beast

Your low back is not fragile! Most of what is supposedly “wrong” with spines and backs is nonsense

Spines haven’t changed in the last century,26 and yet humanity suffers from a great plague of low back pain.27 And, contrary to popular belief, it almost certainly afflicts “primitive” societies just as much as industrialized ones.28 The real causes of most back pain are obscured by medical mythology and misunderstanding,29 and a thick fog of nonsense constantly spewing forth from an army of quacks trying to compensate for medicine’s failure.

Before I discuss what kind of things do cause low back pain, it’s extremely important to talk about what does not cause it. In this section, I will challenge the main myths in just a few paragraphs, supported by dozens of references to the best scientific information available.

Most people — and most health care professionals — believe that back pain is usually caused mainly by structural problems, either injury or degeneration of the spine. This idea is not supported by the scientific evidence.30 Indeed, just the opposite is more the case: “The evidence that tissue pathology does not explain chronic pain is overwhelming (e.g., in back pain, neck pain, and knee osteoarthritis).”31

Spines do degenerate, but not for the reasons most people think they do: genetics is by far the biggest factor in degeneration,32 not your posture, your office chair or mattress, your core stability, or anything else that low back pain sufferers have been taught to blame their pain on.

Belief in spinal fragility and instability is unjustified but deeply held, based on ignorance of a complex subject and on an obsolete mechanical view of biology that has dominated medical thinking for centuries. And when structural problems are exaggerated, you also get a plague of barking-up-the-wrong-tree treatments based on that exaggeration.

END OF FREE INTRODUCTION

Purchase full access to this tutorial for USD$1995. Continue reading this page immediately after purchase. See a complete table of contents below. Most content on PainScience.com is free.?

Almost everything on this website is free: about 80% of the site by wordcount, or 95% of the bigger pages. This page is only one of a few big ones that have a price tag. There are also hundreds of free articles, including several about low back pain. Book sales — over 72,700 since 2007?This is a tough number for anyone to audit, because my customer database is completely private and highly secure. But if a regulatory agency ever said “show us your math,” I certainly could! This count is automatically updated once every day or two, and rounded down to the nearest 100. Due to some oddities in technology over the years, it’s probably a bit of an underestimate. — keep the lights on and allow me to publish everything else (without ads).

Q. Ack, what’s with that surprise price tag?!

A. I know it can make a poor impression, but I have to make a living and this is the best way I’ve found to keep the lights on here.


BUY NOW $1995 USD
Logos for Visa, Mastercard, and Amex.
 ? 

Paying in your own (non-USD) currency is always cheaper! My prices are set slightly lower than current exchange rates, but most cards charge extra for conversion.

Example: as a Canadian, if I pay $19.95 USD, my credit card converts it at a high rate and charges me $26.58 CAD. But if I select Canadian dollars here, I pay only $24.95 CAD.

Why so different? If you pay in United States dollars (USD), your credit card will convert the USD price to your card’s native currency, but the card companies often charge too much for conversion — it’s a way for them to make a little extra money, of course. So I offer my customers prices converted at slightly better than the current rate.

read on any device, no passwords
refund at any time, in a week or a year
call 778-968-0930 for purchase help
ID  Vendor Information
company PainScience.com
owner Paul Ingraham
contact 778-968-0930
refunds 100%, no time limit +Customers are welcome to ask for a refund months after purchase — I understand that it can take time to decide if information like this was worth the price for you.
more info policies page
payments

You can also keep reading more without buying. Here are some other free samples from the book, and other closely related articles on PainScience.com:

BUY NOW $1995 USD
Logos for Visa, Mastercard, and Amex.
 ? 

Paying in your own (non-USD) currency is always cheaper! My prices are set slightly lower than current exchange rates, but most cards charge extra for conversion.

Example: as a Canadian, if I pay $19.95 USD, my credit card converts it at a high rate and charges me $26.58 CAD. But if I select Canadian dollars here, I pay only $24.95 CAD.

Why so different? If you pay in United States dollars (USD), your credit card will convert the USD price to your card’s native currency, but the card companies often charge too much for conversion — it’s a way for them to make a little extra money, of course. So I offer my customers prices converted at slightly better than the current rate.

read on any device, no passwords
refund at any time, in a week or a year
call 778-968-0930 for purchase help

The back pain misinformation explosion

I hope that this page is one of the saner sources of back pain information available online, while also being much more thorough and friendly than the big medical sources. Since I first started treating low back pain in 2000, there’s been an explosion of free online information about it, of course — countless poor quality articles (see Starman). Literally hundreds of thousands of them. Back in the day, we actually had to go to a doctor or buy a book to get shoddy back pain information — now it’s just a Google search away.

Over the years, I’ve collected some interesting examples of just how bad it gets. Here’s three, shared for the chuckles and the teachable moments…

Exhibit A: The worst possible website about pain

Photo of a classy statue doing a “facepalm” of disbelief. The statue is actually Henri Vidal’s depiction of “Cain After Killing His Brother Abel,” on display in the Tuileries Garden in Paris.

ThePain.net is one of the worst examples I’ve seen: a large, stale, ugly thing by Pekka Palin, MD. There are hours of terrible reading there! Dozens of blandly composed, shallow, rambling, and frequently irrational mini-articles on every imaginable pain subject, all 100% unreferenced, laid out in huge blocks of text without a subheading for many screens (a typographic travesty).

There’s soooo much badness, but I really got a chuckle out of this perfectly pointless phrase: “The most common term used for general, temporary low back pain is lumbago.” Now “lumbago” means “back pain” (see the definition of lumbago on Wikipedia), so Dr. Obvious has helpfully explained that the cause of back pain is, er, back pain. The Internet, at your service!

Exhibit B: Awful back pain information from Consumer Reports

Dr. Harriet Hall, critiquing a recent issue of Consumer Reports about back pain:

I found the [Consumer Reports] articles on back pain very disappointing. I hope I can still trust Consumer Reports when shopping for a washing machine, but I have no confidence that I can trust them when looking for an effective medical treatment. They seem not to understand the difference between anecdotes and data, between a popularity contest and a controlled scientific study. These articles may do harm by encouraging readers to try treatments that don’t work and by suggesting that it is reasonable to prioritize testimonial evidence over scientific studies. On the other hand, these articles may do some good insofar as they may dissuade some patients from rushing to a doctor and demanding imaging studies or prescription drugs.

Well, at least there’s that! But most of what CR published was horrifyingly naive and misleading. I scanned this issue in a grocery store lineup and was rolling my eyes within seconds. And then fuming: it seems like the flood of misinformation about back pain is infinite! I’ve been actively debunking back pain myths for about 15 years now, and the need for it has barely changed in all that time. So-called information like this, reaching a massive audience, seriously exacerbates the problem.

I think Consumer Reports has made serious mistakes in other domains over the last few years, and they are quickly burning the credibility it took decades to earn. Sadly, they will still have a strong enough reputation for years to come to do plenty of damage with content like this.

Even many better articles still have serious “attitude” problems. Usually the attitude problem is unjustified optimism. For instance …

Exhibit C: A widely praised “high quality” article full of flaws

This extremely popular 2017 article on Vox.com the “new science” of low back pain was praised by many because it superficially seems to be very modern and science-y, and it correctly dismisses a number of myths, but I think it’s an exasperating failure. It creates a strong impression of being scientifically rigorous without actually being so. It brims with promising science news about alternative treatments that do not actually stand up to more cynical and experienced analysis.

Adding to the façade of scientific credibility, many of the right caveats and disclaimers about the “new science” are technically there — warnings about small effect sizes, mixed evidence, and potential flaws — but these cautions are also belated and consistently understated. The tone is overwhelmingly sunny and naïve, as though we are on the verge of a revolution in back pain treatment thanks to … a bunch of stuff that has been around forever and has clearly not been saving the world from chronic low back pain.

The low point of the article is definitely the advice to try to find a “back whisperer.” Back pain is too difficult a problem for anyone to have that role, no matter how expert and experienced. I think the idea of a back whisperer dangerously promotes false hope.

So who can you trust?

You’d think that you might be able to escape this mess by reading medical journals, but that’s hardly guaranteed. Even back pain guidelines published in medical journals are often misleading. They are not all good. The committees that write these things do not necessarily know the science. One of the best reviews of back pain research ever published — Machado 2009, more on this one later — found something really interesting: “treatment recommendations from recent clinical guidelines do not align with the results of this meta-analysis.” In fact, quite a few disproven pain treatments are still cheerfully recommended in otherwise sensible professional guidelines. Eek. So I am realistic about the limits of the science, much of which is pretty junky. +The quality of science is a huge topic, but here’s one simple example of an extremely common problem with back pain science: control groups that don’t control. Rather than comparing a treatment to a good, carefully selected placebo, most studies use a comparison to a treatment that is allegedly neutral, underwhelming, or placebo-ish. That makes the results hard to interpret: if each works about the same, it could mean that the treatments are equally effective … or equally ineffective! So much back pain science has this problem — or any one of a dozen other weak points — that you can effectively ignore at least 80% of all back pain research, because it’s so far from the last word on anything. Good science is essential to solving these problems, but really good studies are also difficult to design and rare.

So why trust this tutorial? Well, you don’t need to take my word for anything — you can just take the word of the many low back pain medical experts that I quote extensively, and the hard evidence that their opinions are based on.+ (At the same time, I am realistic about the limits of the science, much of which is surprisingly junky.)

Dr. Richard Deyo, low back pain expert

Don’t take my word for anything. Take this guy’s word for it. Dr. Richard Deyo has been busting myths about low back pain for longer than I’ve been alive.

Dr. Nikolai Bogduk, low back pain expert

Or this guy’s word. Dr. Nikolai Bogduk has authored dozens of scientific papers about low back pain. He also questions and challenges many common beliefs about low back pain, and is one of the expert sources this tutorial relies upon.

Dr. Siegfried Mense, muscle pain expert

Or this guy’s. Dr. Siegfried Mense is the world’s foremost expert in muscle pain. He is the author of an authoritative text on that subject, and one of this guide’s more important sources.

And, most importantly of all, I’m not selling a cure: the purpose of this guide is “just” education. It’s best for people who like to understand their problems. Its dorky, quirky thoroughness is unique. I will not just tell you what you want to hear. Understanding low back pain as well as possible is valuable, but it will not necessarily lead to a cure. Sadly, some low back pain cannot be fixed.

BUY NOW $1995 USD
Logos for Visa, Mastercard, and Amex.
 ? 

Paying in your own (non-USD) currency is always cheaper! My prices are set slightly lower than current exchange rates, but most cards charge extra for conversion.

Example: as a Canadian, if I pay $19.95 USD, my credit card converts it at a high rate and charges me $26.58 CAD. But if I select Canadian dollars here, I pay only $24.95 CAD.

Why so different? If you pay in United States dollars (USD), your credit card will convert the USD price to your card’s native currency, but the card companies often charge too much for conversion — it’s a way for them to make a little extra money, of course. So I offer my customers prices converted at slightly better than the current rate.

read on any device, no passwords
refund at any time, in a week or a year
call 778-968-0930 for purchase help

I do worry there is a combination of side effects and unnecessary treatments and labeling people as being fragile when they’re really not. The combination of those kinds of things may actually be in some cases doing more harm than good.

Dr. Richard Deyo, low back pain expert33

Ignoring expert recommendations from their own colleagues,34 doctors order too many fancy tests (especially MRI or “magnetic resonance imaging”)3536373839 to try to find evidence of mechanical failures — yet we know that doing such tests does not lead to better results,40 may even cause harm,41 usually shows problems in people with no pain,42434445464748 and shows no problems in people who do have pain!495051

None of these mean that the cause of pain can’t still be lurking in the spine — it could be.52 But either the causes of back pain that can be seen on MRI scans are surprisingly rare, or hard to spot, or both.

Photograph of an MRI machine in a luxurious modern clinic.

Medical care for the Jetsons?

For many years now, MRI scans have been the ultimate in futuristic medicine. But while these machines are miraculous in some ways, they can be worse than useless for diagnosing low back pain & studies show that doctors recommend way too many of them … especially when they profit from it.

The overdiagnosis of slipped discs in particular

With and without MRI, intervertebral disc herniation is the most overdiagnosed cause of back pain, probably one of the most overdiagnosed problems in the history of medicine. Herniations are feared by patients way out of proportion to their frequency or severity.53 Herniation severity does not correlate well with pain in the first place, but they definitely can’t be the cause of the trouble when they just go away on their own.

Once a spinal disc has “slipped,” it seems like it’s in a biomechanically awkward situation and can’t recover any more than a broken window can reassemble itself. And yet “slipped” discs usually un-slip! This is called “resorption” — a nifty back trick that most people are unaware of (including too many healthcare professionals still). Most herniations, roughly 60%, just go away, some of them surprisingly quickly, like a snail tucking back into its shell, according to about a dozen studies.54 Not only that, but it’s actually the worst ones that are the most likely to resolve on their own!55 One of the best of those studies tracked herniations for eight years, finding that only 12% got worse.56 You can see a good example of resorption in these reassuring before/after pictures (follow link in footnote).57 And here’s another eyebrow-raiser: a study in New England Journal of Medicine showed that people with back pain and herniations actually recovered slightly better than those with “normal” discs!58 For every disc that recovers completely, more probably resolve enough to relieve a key symptom.

And yet it is almost impossible emotionally to see anything that looks “bad” on an X-ray or MRI and not worry about it. And it’s also almost impossible to get a completely healthy-looking scan of anyone! TV’s Dr. Greg House has commented on this …

House: Give him a whole body scan.

Cameron: You hate whole body scans.

House: Because they’re useless. You could probably scan every one of us and find five different doodads that look like cancer.

House, Season 1, Episode 17, Role Model, written by Matt Whitten

Inconsistently wrong

But even Dr. House wasn’t cynical enough: radiologists can’t even spot the same red herrings. If you send the same patient to get ten different MRIs, interpreted by ten different radiologists from different facilities, you will get ten markedly different explanations for her symptoms. A 63-year-old volunteer with sciatica allowed herself to be scanned again and again and again for science59 — a sting operation. The results were all over the map.

The radiologists cooked up forty-nine distinct “findings.” Sixteen were unique; not one was found in all ten reports, and only one was found in nine of the ten. On average, each radiologist made about a dozen errors, seeing one or two things that weren’t there and missing about ten things that were. That’s a lot of errors, and near-zero reliability. The authors clearly believe that some MRI providers are better than others, and that’s probably true, but we also need to ask the question: is any MRI reliable? This would be laughable if it weren’t so tragic.

Most anatomical defects are clinical red herrings, about as scary as a birthmark, and signify nothing except the power of modern medical technology to scare you silly if you aren’t armed to the teeth with confident knowledge to the contrary.

Over-medicalization of treatment as well as diagnosis

Continuing on the theme of over-medicalization of back pain, doctors also prescribe far too much surgery and way too many drugs,6061 even though we have evidence that these approaches to the problem routinely have disappointing results.6263646566 Even when surgery appears to work, the benefits may often be due to placebo effect rather than correction of a significant problem.67

Many years before when I had serious back pain from a sports injury, the surgeons said they would explore my spine and “figure it out.” Out of frustration I had impulsively opted for the procedure. They ended up fusing the vertebrae. It left me debilitated. In hindsight, I blamed myself more than the surgeons. I had pressed them for a solution when in fact none was apparent because the cause of the pain was obscure.

How doctors think, by Jerome Groopman

The truth is that your back is not fragile — it is a strong, adaptable structure.68 Multiple spinal joints can be fused without causing pain or loss of function.69 The big muscles around the spine can be quite asymmetrical with no ill effect.70 The spine can heal from injuries as well as any other part of the body, and can adapt well to significant mechanical failures — study after study has shown that people also can have significant spinal degeneration, deformities, and injuries without any pain.71727374757677

There is even strong and recent evidence that structural stability is not a critical factor in the pain of collapsed vertebrae — stabilizing those bones with injected cement doesn’t make them recover any faster, as surgeons had reasonably assumed for decades.78

Even serious structural problems in the low back are often painless

Even spondylolisthesis and stenosis — true dislocations of vertebrae, and narrowing of the tube that your spinal cord lives in — may not be the primary cause of pain in many cases.79 Until relatively recently in history, almost everyone (except John Sarno80) assumed that spinal stenosis was painful. It’s spinal-canal narrowing! Ouch! Right? That’s gotta hurt!

[MRI scan of a severe spondylolisthesis in a patient who has never actually had significant pain.]

This is an MRI scan of a severe spondylolisthesis — a dislocation of the lowest lumbar joint. This patient has never had low back pain.

But in 2006, Haig et al published truly surprising evidence that spinal stenosis often does not hurt.81 In this study, about 150 people were assessed for back pain either with MRI alone or just with physical assessment. MRI can certainly detect narrowing of the spinal canal, but on the basis of MRI alone, doctors could not identify which patients were hurting, because many of the people who had stenosis did not have pain. These results also strongly imply that a narrowed spinal canal does not (alone) cause back pain.

Consider this striking case, of one of the most memorable clients I ever had as a massage therapist. The picture is an MRI of a type of dislocation, a bad spondylolisthesis (grade III) — her fifth lumbar vertebrae was basically falling off her sacrum, slipping down and forward. It was about 50% displaced. The degree of structural deformity seemed extreme, almost disastrously so. How could she even walk? And that has got to be painful, right?

Incredibly, this middle-aged client was not a back pain client; she sought my assistance for a minor foot problem. She’d never had significant low back pain in her life: only occasional minor episodes. As if that were not surprising enough, she also had severe scoliosis. This is not an isolated case of a serious but painless dislocation; there are other case studies of relatively asymptomatic dislocation and deformity.8283 I discuss them in more detail in an article about spinal manipulative therapy.

Of course, spondylolisthesis, spinal stenosis, and other extreme anatomical situations undoubtedly can cause pain. Stenotic pain, for instance, is responsible for the characteristic forward stoop of many elderly persons — because stenosis causes pain when you straighten up, it forces its victims to bend forward to escape it. It’s a real problem. But it’s also fascinating that they cause so much less pain (or other problems) than anyone previously thought.

Clearly something beyond the obvious is going on.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Maybe you’re just getting older? Actually, no …

You are getting older! But getting older is probably not the reason your back hurts. This is an important clue that back pain often cannot be blamed on biomechanical glitches.

Many kinds of pain are known to increase steadily with age. Any doctor with some experience knows this. And every person over the age of thirty-five can feel it. And it seems consistent with the common sense idea that spinal degeneration progresses with age and causes pain.

After the age of 40, perfectly normal vertebral columns rapidly become rarer and rarer. It is unusual after that age to see spines without x-ray evidence of aging, including thinning of disks and thinning of articular surfaces. The longer a man lives, the more impressive the radiologic changes in his vertebral column become.

College of Physicians and Surgeons of the Province of Quebec84

Yet young people — working people in their thirties and even twenties — get severe back pain in numbers so large that they are routinely highlighted for their economic significance.

And there is evidence that people actually get less back pain as they age! Certain types of back pain are relatively absent in the young and become increasingly common with age, especially some of the most severe kinds of back pain — more on these examples below. But those are the exceptions that prove the rule.

The overall trend is clear: most typical back pain occurs in the thirties and forties,85 either actually declining in the fifties and sixties,86 or at least not steadily increasing — exactly the opposite of what you’d expect if back pain was mainly caused by the degeneration that we know is occurring!8788 Just look at this camel-hump of a graph of one survey:89

Graph of the percentage of survey respondents who reported low back pain at different ages.

That’s a striking prevalence of low back pain in mid-life … and a striking decline later in life. Not every data set shows this so clearly — I confess to cherry-picking one of the most impressive examples — but even where there isn’t such a clear decline, any decline in pain with age, or even just a lack of increase, is sharply at odds with the conventional wisdom.

If low back pain were about spinal “wear-and-tear,” you would not only expect to see more back pain in older people, but worse back pain in older people — much worse. If spines were fragile, and if their fragility was driving low back pain, then the thirty-year-old back pain patient, on average, would be much better off than the sixty-year-old back pain patient. And yet this is decidedly not the case! Many younger patients with no history of injury suffer from extremely severe pain — pain that is well out of proportion to the degeneration that cannot possibly be significantly afflicting their spines at their age.

Fun of a spry older man kicking his leg up very high, representing how surprisingly rare back pain is in seniors.

Degenerating, schmegenerating!

Although they aren’t immune, older people are actually relatively free of low back pain. It’s the thirty- & fortysomethings that generate the horrifying low back pain statistics.

None of this is widely known, but it’s not out in left field. We know that degeneration correlates poorly with pain, and is mostly determined by genetics and pathology anyway, and not “primarily through routine physical loading exposures (eg, heavy vs. light physical demands) as once suspected.”90 Although family doctors may not be aware of this research, they can learn it easily enough from medical experts, just like I have. For instance, Dr. Richard Deyo of Seattle, one of the world’s foremost medical authorities on back pain, knows full well that back pain is not usually structural, and he’s done much of the research that proves it. In his tutorial for doctors published in the New England Journal of Medicine in 2001, Deyo makes it clear to physicians that only a small amount of back pain has any structural cause, and the rest of it — about 70 to 85% — is just unexplained.9192

The nastiest types of back pain do increase with age

Certain types of back pain are relatively absent in the young and then surge with age. Deyo explains that “cancer, compression fractures, spinal stenosis, and aortic aneurysms become more common.” And Vlaeyen et al emphasized that “if only more severe forms are included, the prevalence steadily increased in individuals ≥65 years of age,”93 based on Dionne et al (which is actually a paper entirely devoted to this question).94

Cancer is a solid example, because it definitely gets more common with age, and it sure can cause severe back pain, so that’s going to drive up the number of cases of nasty back pain in older people. But if you subtract such cases from consideration, then the lack of any clear sign of non-specific back pain worsening with age is even more glaring.

Dionne et al. concluded that it’s “unclear” how age affects non-specific low back pain. My opinion is that the absence of clarity is precisely what’s interesting. If aging is bad for backs, why isn’t it more obvious?

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Diagnose, schmiagnose! Structural problems in the low back are (very) hard to diagnose accurately

It’s hard to find biomechanical bogeymen — even when they do exist. Even when structural abnormalities that matter actually are haunting a back, healthcare professionals have an extremely difficult time accurately identifying them in an exam. The ability to pick up on subtle signs with advanced palpation skills and diagnostic cleverness and sophistication is something that essentially all physical therapists aspire to. It is almost the basis of some professions, like chiropractic, where the whole point is to “correct” spinal dysfunctions … which must first be identified, of course. Similarly, massage therapists cannot “release” knots if they cannot find them. And physical therapists cannot prescribe specific corrective exercises without picking a target.

The difficulty does not seem to stop anyone from guessing. If I had a buck for every time I heard something like this…

“It’s my sacroiliac joint. My physiotherapist told me.”

Patients get told this kind of thing a lot.

The sacroiliac joint is an especially popular scapegoat, but there are many reasons to doubt that the sacroliac joint is a cause of pain nearly as often as it is diagnosed, beyond the fact that there are just so many possibilities. For instance, consider its legendary toughness:

I talked to a trauma surgeon that had been to a workshop where they talked about the sacrum being “out of place.” He just said this is ridiculous: we see people who are in motor vehicle accidents with every bit of their body smashed but the sacroiliac joint is intact. It is so strong.

Peter O’Sullivan, Professor of Musculoskeletal Physiotherapy at Curtin University, Perth, Australia

So patients are routinely given specific pathoanatomical explanations for their back pain, and it always sounds impressive … mostly just because it’s “specific” and “pathoanatomical.” It sounds good. Medical jargon makes things sound Serious.

Surely there are tests that can identify the problem tissue?

In a 2000 editorial for the Medical Journal of Australia, Dr. Nikolai Bogduk sassily wrote that many diagnoses for low back pain are “illegitimate, inappropriate, or fanciful95 (italics mine) and explained that there is hope for more precise diagnosis … but only with specific medical tests, such as nerve blocks and provocative discography.

Is there really, though? There might be a little “hope” in such testing, but the evidence shows that common diagnostic tests for back pain just don’t work very well — not even the more hardcore options like nerve blocks.

In 2007, the European Spine Journal published an excellent paper about the ability of professionals to suss out the specific cause of back pain using a variety of “specific medical tests.” This scientific study of about 40 tests-of-testing studies put the claims to the test!96 (Yes, that sentence does actually make sense.) Can the pros really pinpoint where low back pain is coming from?

Nope. Some are surely better than others, but the average is just really poor.

At best, testing only modestly increased the odds of correctly identifying the source of pain. “Some diagnostic value” is the strongest positive assessment in the article — and that refers primarily to MRI being used to help confirm a disc as the source of pain (not eliminating it, which they cannot do).97

Facet joints are a common scapegoat for back pain, but (emphasis mine) “none of the tests for facet joint pain were found to be informative.”98 This makes me cringe, because I “tested” for facet joint involvement frequently during my clinical career. That was a couple minutes of my patients’ time and money wasted every time I did it (to say nothing of the misleading results). And the evidence that such testing is largely futile had already existed for years even back then, but had not trickled down to me through my texts, instructors, or continuing education. Yet another great example of how important it is for clinicians to keep up with their journal reading.

The effectiveness of common kinds of testing for spinal dysfunction was “at best moderate,” and that was only if multiple tests were used. The tests bombed when tested independently. No one test was any good by itself. Such testing involves a lot of skill and knowledge, so to get even “moderate” results you would also have to have the most competent testing, a top-of-class type of doctor or therapist — rare by definition.99

One would hope that common diagnostic tests for spinal trouble would produce reasonably unambiguous results which, when checked for their reliability, would prove to be correct most of the time. Instead what we have is a mess of underwhelming performance not much better than pointing at random anatomy while blindfolded.

Hancock et al added to the (already large) pile of evidence that “conventional investigations do not reveal the cause of [low back] pain”100 (that’s Dr. Bogduk again). In 2009, the American Pain Society also gave a thumbs down to provocative discography in their official guidelines for low back pain — reducing the diagnostic options even more.101

Similarly dismal results trying to identify bad facet joints in the neck

King et al piled on with similar conclusions about the reliability of tests for the source of neck pain — it “lacks validity.”102

They compared physiotherapist diagnoses of 173 people to more reliable testing done by blocking the joint. What they found was that physical therapists were good at identifying facet joints that were painful — which is great, good for them! — but they were also rather prone to diagnosing a facet joint issue even when there was no pain. In other words, they were over-diagnosing, telling practically everyone that they had facet joint pain, which was correct in some cases, but wrong in many others. In spite of this, “Manual therapists believe that they can diagnose symptomatic joints in the neck by manual examination,” they wrote.

Can’t touch this

Maigne et al tested two physicians with training in manual medicine to see if they could detect the painful side by feeling for tension in the spinal muscles. In almost two hundred patients, they identified the correct side of 65% of lower back pain and 59% of neck pain — barely better than chance.103

And, if they couldn’t do it, most massage therapists would probably fail the same test — despite our legendary reputation for zeroing in on tissue problems with uncanny accuracy.

A little humility, please!

I went through a phase for a few years where I started to worry that other healthcare professionals were much smarter than I am, because they seem so good at identifying exactly where back pain is coming from. (Also, they still tell me they are.) But years of experience and study have taught me that humility and restraint may be wiser, and less wrong, than most of these “specific pathanatomical explanations.” From Ben Cormack, having a bit of a rant about pseudoscience and amateurism in back pain diagnosis:

“The worlds best researchers admit they can’t reliably diagnose [back pain], so why can you?”

People (patients and pros) do constantly talk like they “know” what causes back pain, or at least have a very strong suspicion, and it’s almost always delusional. The specific mechanisms of back pain are mostly as impossible to know as what Bilbo had in his pocketses. We cannot generally trust professionals to identify a structural origin for your pain, even there is one. Which there may well not be.

We need a lot more diagnostic humility.

None of this means that specific causes don’t exist, and I’ll be talking about this more soon in the chapter, “It’s not structure, except when it is: “specific” back pain.” But the evidence strongly hints that these causes you can point to on an anatomy chart are not present as much as anyone thinks … and even when they are, they’re fiendishly hard to confirm … and not as severe and consistently problematic as people assume and fear.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Those scary spine models

The scary spine models are a great example of how old ideas in health care persist. And make trouble.

Model of doom & gloom

Lumbar spine models like this almost all show a herniated disc. Some go a step further & show discs slipped so far “they’ve completely left the spine”! (Hat tip to Dr. Lorimer Moseley.)

Many experts have made strong statements about the importance of reassuring back pain patients … and not horrifying them with ominous diagnoses like “herniated disc.” Such diagnoses are usually wrong — or hopelessly oversimplified at best — and needlessly intimidate patients … which is itself a risk factor for low back pain. Nervous low back pain patients tend to have more pain for much longer.

And yet it remains almost impossible for a clinician to buy an anatomical model of the lumbar spine that doesn’t have a little rubber disc bulging ominously from the spine … invariably coloured bright red, just to hammer the point home!

And it remains almost impossible for a patient to look at such a model without thinking, “Yikes! That’s what’s going on in my back?”

That little bulging disc looks bad. Or, God forbid, a disc that has “slipped so far out it’s sitting on its own”? Here’s pain researcher Lorimer Moseley making this point in a great TED talk. He’s talking about how pain is probably intensified by the belief that there is danger — the so-called nocebo, the opposite of a placebo — and plastic anatomical models of slipped discs are much too persuasive.

Any piece of credible evidence that they are in danger should change their pain … And they are all walking into a hospital department with models like this on the desk: what does your brain say when it sees a disc that’s slipped so far out it’s sitting on its own? If you’ve ever seen a disc in a cadaver, you can’t slip the suckers — they’re immobile, you can’t slip a disc — but that’s our language, and it messes with your brain. It cannot not mess with your brain.

Lorimer Moseley, from his surprisingly funny TED talk, Why Things Hurt 14:33

(I laughed out loud at that, and then cheered. I’ve been bitching about these blasted models for many years.)

Such models also undoubtedly also influence professionals. Even if clinicians accept that it’s an oversimplified model, the prominence of herniated discs in most models and anatomical drawings constantly exaggerates their importance.

Anatomical models aren’t cheap, and once a clinician has purchased one, it will likely stay in his or her office for many years, even decades. I’m sure there are probably hundreds of thousands of these things in offices around the world that are at least twenty years old. And clinicians are still buying new ones today!

And so this is a great example of how hopelessly obsolete clinical ideas persist for years, even decades, after the field has moved on.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

It’s not structure, except when it is: “specific” back pain

The medical jargon for back pain without a specific explanation is “non-specific” back pain. It’s what we say when we can’t be any more specific than “it’s back pain!” It almost like saying “undiagnosed” or “baffling” back pain… and it’s not really clear why we don’t just say that.

This “non-specific” term is odd. It isn’t standard for any other medical condition; there is no non-specific headache, for instance. A headache without a specific diagnosis is traditionally called a “tension” headache, which is even worse, because it presumes a cause, which is probably usually wrong. Imagine if we called all puzzling back pain “tension back pain.”

Chronic low back pain is often classified as non-specific: that is, no organic cause of the pain can be identified. This happens a lot.

But it’s not the default! Back pain usually gets blamed on a specific cause prematurely, without actually knowing that it’s correct — much like the assumption that an undiagnosed headache is caused by “tension” (when we don’t even know for sure that tension can explain any headache, and there are many other known causes). Three common examples:

  1. Many professionals still default to assuming that undiagnosed back pain is “degenerative” in some way, with variable specificity and vehemence, and that is the problematic assumption that I have mostly focused on so far in this book.
  2. The most popular specific explanation for back pain among patients is probably “spasm,” which is more or less exactly the same as blaming headaches on tension. It isn’t terribly “specific,” but it is certainly more specific than “no bloody clue.” It is an idea! It is a specific hypothetical mechanism for pain. And it cannot be overstated how easily people take it seriously as the explanation for a new attack of back pain. I have personally heard people casually attribute back pain to spasm at least several hundred times in my career.
  3. And then there’s a specific explanation beloved more or less equally by both patients and pros: the assumption of injury — that some relatively innocuous physical stress is the culprit. For a specific explanation, it isn’t terribly specific: exactly what anatomy has gotten hurt, and how, is often not specified by this specific diagnosis. “Something in the spine got hurt somehow” is as specific as it gets… or perhaps “slipped disc,” because that’s s the only specific injury that many people have even heard of.

All of these things are possible specific explanations for back pain, but none are them safe assumptions, not by a long shot.

That unsafe assumption has an evil twin: the equally unsafe assumption that there is no specific cause, which is implied by the label “non-specific back pain.” Rather than simply being unknown, too many people (mainly clinicians) slip into overconfidently assuming that no specific cause actually exists — or that it is so perfectly unknowable that it might as well not exist.

The other side of the coin: just because we don’t know the specific diagnosis doesn’t mean it is isn’t there

“…degenerative disc disease is undervalued as a cause of chronic low-back pain by clinicians, patients and society.”

Rustenburg et al, 2018, JOR Spine

Sometimes structure does matter. I have spilled a lot of “ink” in this book about the urgent need to back away from the pervasive idea that back pain is a structural problem (especially degenerative disc disease). I have demonstrated that specific, structural causes of back pain are over-emphasized, but not non-existent. Like violent crimes, they are far less common than the news makes people fear, but they do still happen.

In 2015, a perfect pair of papers by the same researchers pushed each way on this topic: one away from mechanical explanations… the other right towards them. The first was cited above (along with many other similar ones): Brinjikji et al looked at a whole lot of MRI pictures of spines, and presented evidence that signs of spinal degeneration are present in amazingly high percentages of healthy people with no problem at all. Good to know. It’s one of the best papers of that sort, highlighting the weird disconnect between back pain and spinal degeneration.

But Brinjikji et al didn’t stop there! Their second paper presented evidence that degenerative features visible on MRI are nevertheless “more prevalent in adults 50 years of age or younger with back pain compared with asymptomatic individuals.”104

Also good to know!

These papers aren’t actually at odds. The take-home message of the pair is just a nice, reasonable, and obvious compromise: degenerative changes matter less than many patients and professionals still assume, and are not an adequate foundation for many popular treatments… but they do still matter.

Duh? Surely no one is surprised by this!

Importantly, Brinjikji et al also showed that the disconnect between structure and pain is much less striking for some kinds of problems than others. Intervertebral joint dislocation (spondylolisthesis) is rarely asymptomatic! Some cases are, and that’s fascinating. But not most. 60% with bulging discs at age 50 are symptom free, but only 14% of 50-year-olds with spondylolisthesis are feeling no pain. It’s amazing that anyone can have a dislocated spinal joint and feel fine, but 85% do have symptoms.

So whether structure matters depends. Some spinal problems matter more than others. Some of them are worse than others. Duh again.

Neuroma: a very specific example of a very specific cause of back pain

Obvious sources of acute and/or ominous pain like infections, aneurysms, tumours, fractures, and inflammatory diseases are all causes of some back pain, but they aren’t of much interest here because they are so serious (and often relentlessly progressive) that they mostly do get diagnosed. They are the kinds of things that mostly qualify as “specific” back pain.

But what about specific problems that tend to drag on and never get diagnosed? Sure there are also specific causes of chronic low back pain that get missed? Of course there are. The only real question is: how often does that happen? No one really knows.

A friend of mine — let’s call him Alex — suffered for about five years from slowly but steadily worsening back pain before a wee little tumour was finally discovered: it was “benign” as far as tumours go, but it was growing on a nerve in his back (a “neuroma”), and it had just about ruined him. Neuromas are tumours, but they aren’t the kind that kill you … they just make you miserable. My friend had dropped out of the sport he loved, ultimate, the Frisbee sport, which is how I know him — we had played for years together on a team called Afternoon Delight. (Fun fact: Our team symbol was a pair of humping unicorns.)

At his worst, Alex could barely walk. Eventually, after an easy surgery, he was entirely cured, just like that. Boom. Specific as it gets.

Alex had seen many, many healthcare professionals who had failed to diagnose the real problem, thrown up their hands, and chalked it all up to non-specific back pain. I know, because I was one of those healthcare professionals.105

Before the diagnosis, “sensitization” had been a major diagnostic option, a classic pseudo-diagnosis that means the problem is a false alarm. Although Alex likely was suffering from the phenomenon of sensitization, it was probably a sideshow, a complication of the real source of serious irritation in his back.

A chiropractor finally suggested the right kind of spinal imaging, which easily identified the neuroma. Once everyone knew it was there, that was the end of any discussions about how strange and unexplained chronic back pain is! He just needed surgery to remove a little blob of uninvited tissue from a nerve root. And that surgery worked immediately and permanently.

That’s probably the single clearest example I’ve ever encountered of a specific cause of back pain, no fine print. The next example is also quite good, but it is not as cut and dried.

🎶 M-m-m-my Neuroma
M-m-m-my Neuroma
M-m-m-my Neuroma
M-m-m-my Neuroma
Ohhhhh my Neuroma
Ohhhhh my Neuroma
Ohhhhh my Neuroma 🎵

Cluneal nerve entrapment: another specific example, and another slam dunk surgical fix (maybe)

The cluneal nerves pass from the low back and sacrum into the buttocks, just under the skin, and they can get “tangled up” with ligaments and connective tissue on their way (nerve entrapment). The irritation potentially causes low back and leg pain. Most cases are probably relatively minor and are obviously related to a peripheral nerve in distress: that is, some cases probably don’t feel like classic back pain, but rather back pain with an obviously nervy quality (superficial, electrical, with some tingling and other distorted sensation). The location of the pain is probably also a little on the low side, less “back” than sacroiliac joint, butt pain, and a touch of sciatica.

In 2016, Aota reported on “a case of severe low back pain, which was completely treated by release of the middle cluneal nerve.”106 The patient suffered from nasty “back and buttock pain radiating to both legs” which was “continuous and severe,” and had come on gradually over a decade. She did also have some prominent tingling, however, so neurological involvement was probably always suspected. But she’d been operated on already — a clean miss, a pointless diskectomy to relieve nerve root pressure that didn’t exist.

She was lucky enough to have a doctor who figured it out and talked her into something that I doubt I would have agreed to: exploratory surgery to identify nerves “entrapped in adhesions.” Freeing it up definitely helped part of the problem, but it didn’t solve it. It took a second attempt to find the main problem, a leap of faith that I would definitely have avoided. But it’s a good thing she took it!

They found a tiny spot where the nerve passed through a ligament, cut it free, and that was the ticket: she was decisively cured. Which is pretty cool.

#1 marks the site of the superior cluneal nerves & where the first surgery freed nerves from “adhesions.” #2 marks the middle cluneal nerve, where the second surgery freed it from entrapment in the ligament. Click to embiggen

That’s about as structural a problem as we can imagine. Tissue in trouble! As physical as snarled sailboat rigging.

And yet it is still also completely possible or even likely that her problem was not the nerve “snag” per se but a biological vulnerability to feeling it. The physical predicament of the nerve may have been like kindling for a fire — a fire that was then lit by something else (and which then burned for years).107

Even if that was the case — and it would certainly be fascinating — diagnosing the vulnerability and solving it might be much trickier than just setting the nerve free. No kindling, no fire! Mostly this problem seems as straightforwardly mechanical as your legs falling asleep because a 40-pound terrier won’t get off your lap. Sure, maybe that dog will be a problem for you more and sooner than someone else… but it’s still easier to just shoo the dog off your lap than it is to figure out why it’s such a problem for you. If you can handle the sad puppy dog eyes.

There’s “no such thing” as non-specific back pain?

Back pain expert Dr. Stuart McGill has famously argued that there is “no such thing” as non-specific back pain: “there are only those individuals who have not had a thorough assessment.”108 He says this because of the tricky cases like neuromas, annular lesions, and cluneal nerve impingement. In a podcast interview, he gave the following interesting example:

MRIs are static, but when you watch dynamics of spine movement, things really change. The instabilities show up, the micromovements show up. In my last few years at the university, we did fluoroscopic investigations of whiplash patients. [X-ray “movies.” See digital motion X-ray. A powerful tool, rarely justified for a variety of reasons.] These were patients who were rather summarily dismissed by typical medical practice, the radiology reports said “oh, there’s nothing [specific] wrong with you.” Well, when we followed them and watched them move with fluoroscopy, we would see that they could move through the range of motion, and let’s say for example as they passed through 10 degrees of flexion they would get a shot of pain. We would watch and quantify on the dynamic fluoro image a sheer movement of the person’s neck. The legacy of the whiplash was a local joint laxity and instability and that would … cause pain.

So here was this poor person dismissed by MRI and yet they had a very specific and quantifiable mechanism for their pain. So you can imagine the cognitive dissonance that person was now in because some medical authority questioned the legitimacy of what they felt every day.

Dr. Stu McGill, interview for Evidence In Motion

Interesting! But again, just as with the nerve entrapment thing, maybe the problem in such patients was less “mechanical” than it looked, and could only be understood in a larger, messier context of physiological vulnerability. Everyone is easily seduced by the apparent explanatory power of a correlation on MRI. This story seems compelling, but it’s not really any different in principle from the well-established mistake of assuming that a picture of a herniated disc confirms the cause. The picture is compelling in both cases. A picture is worth a thousand words … but what if the words are lies? Both kinds of pictures seem to scream, “Look at me! I’m obviously the problem!” In one case we know it’s often misleading, so why assume the other is a slam dunk? It is a tighter correlation, but it’s still just a correlation. The inference of causality is reasonable, but it clearly ain’t a slam dunk either.

Dr. McGill believes a diagnosable biophysical cause is often there and can be found if we only look hard and smart enough. I admire his confidence! I have no doubt that some back pain mysteries can be found by a good enough medical Sherlock Holmes, but that leaves all the others: I doubt that anyone is actually capable of finding the problem in many other cases, and the quest to find a mechanical cause usually turns into a wild goose chase.

Unfortunately, I also know that it’s hard to find “a good enough medical Sherlock holmes.” In practice, even if all back pain is actually specific, it’s so hard to pin the specific tail on the diagnostic donkey that it barely exists as an possibility in the real world.

A large family of specific possibilities (with one major neglected one)

It may well be possible to find a proximate cause “if we look hard and smart enough” (like cluneal nerve entrapment), but those causes probably routinely not the only cause. That janky vertebral movement may be the specific trigger for pain, but it’s probably resting on other layers of causation (such as biological vulnerabilities, like being a little too inflamed… which is, for example, probably a fairly common and subtle complication of viral infections109).

I also think it’s obvious that many cases are in turn caused by something else. The true opposite to “non-specific” back pain is not a singular specific cause, but multiple causes, none of which can make much trouble all by itself… but put them together and you might get a rogue wave of pain that lasts until one or more of those something else’s backs off.

One of those something-else causes is, I believe, both extremely common and widely underestimated, something many professionals aren’t even looking for. I think that topic deserves a whole bunch of special attention, and so the next several sections will focus on “muscle pain.” But then I’ll return to a long list of other possible specific causes and factors: muscle strain, spasm, overuse injury, disc herniations, facet joint trouble, subluxation, sacroliac joint dysfunction, aligment problems like short legs, pelvic tilts, obesity, core weakness, foot wonkiness, pinched nerves, scoliosis, and — last but not least — the spectre of back mice. 🐭

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

So then what? A plausible missing piece of the back pain puzzle

If we have to toss most of the conventional wisdom about back pain out the door, what replaces it? If most back pain cannot be explained with a specific pathology — or not just one of them, anyway — what can we attribute it to? If the usual suspects rarely actually hurt, or don’t hurt that much, then what the heck does hurting so persistently?

Ideally it would be something we could do something about. Or at least try. Without risking much expense or harm.

I propose that a fair bit of puzzling low back pain is either caused and/or significantly complicated primarily by “cranky” muscle tissue, which may respond surprisingly well to modest doses of massage (among a few other options).

This is not a hypothesis I make lightly, and I know (all too well) that this idea has been seriously criticized by some experts. But, happily, this hypothesis does not have to be proven to be helpful for many people: it is enough for it to be somewhat plausible and relatively cheap and safe to try. Experimental medicine, yes, but not dangerous (as long as you don’t do anything reckless in pursuit of relief).

The main Big Idea of this book is that a lot of low back pain is probably110 basically just muscle pain in the low back. The point is not that this is The Answer or the What Doctors Don’t Want You To Know About Back Pain or some such nonsense. I have chosen it as the major focus in this book because it’s one of the most useful hypotheses that many people are either entirely unaware of, or underestimate. It’s a good-bang-for-buck educational focus.

The idea is that the problem is not the spine, but around the spine. Not pinched nerve root pain, not herniated disc pain, not dislocation or subluxation pain, not arthritic facet joint pain, not torn muscle pain, not even a “muscle spasm” (even though all of these things can occasionally be the undiagnosed culprit). Not anything specific that is commonly diagnosed (or feared) by patients or professionals.

Just the humble muscle “knot” — a poetic and imperfect word for a quirky and controversial clinical entity.

Introducing trigger points

The more official word for a muscle knot is a “trigger point,” and a collection of particularly nasty trigger points is often (rather hand-wavingly) myofascial pain syndrome (MPS). Trigger points and MPS are important concepts and a major focus in this tutorial because:

  • There are many well-established ideas about trigger points, at least partially supported by plenty of interesting scientific evidence slowly accumulating over decades. It’s definitely a half-baked mess,111 but it’s also not way out in left field either. It’s not even remotely as snake oily as homeopathy or psychic healing. For instance …
  • The existence of unexplained sensitive spots in soft tissue is not in itself a matter for debate. Nearly everything else about them is hotly debated, but not their existence. The fiercest critics of the conventional wisdom concede that we are all studying what people subjectively experience as pain in muscle.112 There may be a great deal about them that we don’t understand, and they may be difficult to diagnose and treat, but they are there, and they have some distinctive properties (properties often observed in back pain).
  • Trigger points seem to occur in the low back in unusual numbers and severity. Although there are several regions in the body where trigger points are common, the low back seems by far the most prone to it.113
  • The clinical importance of trigger points is based on an unholy triple threat: they do not just (1) cause pain, they also (2) complicate any other pain problem, and (3) mimic other pain problems. And so they are likely to affect nearly every case of back pain at least a little, more or less regardless of whatever else might be going on. This cannot be said of other factors in back pain. For instance, discs do not herniate in response to muscle pain! But muscle probably does start to hurt due to disc herniations … and then continue after the herniation resolves, which they generally do.
  • Best of all, trigger points are a Good News diagnosis insofar as their bark is worse than their bite: they are usually self-limiting and somewhat treatable, however clumsy and imprecise our methods. They may not be responsible for all back pain, but they are probably responsible for a bunch of it, and responsible for the part of it that we can work with relatively cheaply and safely.

The bark of trigger points may be worse than their bite, but they can bark very loudly. They can be amazingly painful — even frighteningly so, much worse and more persistent than an actual injury, probably far more painful than you would ever expect a mere muscle knot to be. This is one of their distinctive properties that is similar to back pain, which is so notorious for causing so much misery without any easily detected tissue trouble.

⚠️️ Anecdote zone: my professional experience treating back pain with trigger point therapy

I estimate that about trigger points are a major factor in “many” cases of mild to moderate unexplained low back pain. Much garden variety chronic back pain is probably just some trigger points that got a little out of hand. The relationship between trigger points and the pain is often so straightforward that the correct therapy is relatively easy — you find one or two key sensitive spots, stimulate them a bit, and bam, you’re a “miracle worker.”

And in many cases patients are just as capable of working this miracle as a healthcare professional with at least basic knowledge of trigger point therapy.

For instance, consider one patient who’d suffered from moderate, chronic back pain for several years. She’d been given several predictable structural misdiagnoses in that time, especially sacroiliac joint dysfunction, one of the classic back bogeymen. But she had a prominent sore spot in the top edge of her gluteus maximus that, when stimulated, produced the same sensation as her symptoms: a deep, sickly, nagging ache in the region of the low back and upper gluteals. In three appointments of moderate rubbing of that spot and a few adjacent ones, years of pain was completely relieved. Boom. This made me a “miracle worker” in her mind. She was quite surprised:

Just wanted to give you a quick update … my back has been absolutely fine. Unbelievable … or perhaps not, considering what I’ve learned from you! A big thank you for all your help.

Lois McConnell, retired airline executive, suffered chronic low back and hip pain for a few years

I have many such stories. In almost every such case, the pain was not terrible, but frustrating and persistent for years, then relieved by a handful of treatments — far sooner than natural recovery seems likely to have occurred. For a manual therapist, it doesn’t get much better than that. I’m shy about writing it down, because skeptics are absolutely correct to be skeptical of such anecdotes.

However, this whole idea is not too good to be true: it’s just ordinary good.

A willingness to focus on muscle seemed to give me the chance to help a surprising number of pain problems that would otherwise have defied me … but by no means was I actually working miracles, and failure was also common.114 I’m definitely not bragging that I was ever able to entirely fix any case of back pain in three appointments! A more realistic boast would have been: “I can significant improve about 60% of cases of moderate chronic low back pain of a certain type with two to six hours of massage. Ish.” (And I’m consciously correcting for all kinds of nasty biases that make clinicians overestimate their own effectiveness.)

Not miraculous, just good. Better than average.

And so it became my cautious suspicion back in the 2000s that back pain is often mainly about trigger points. And it has remained so even through the 2010s and now into the 2020s, as this idea came under real skeptical siege — in some cases from professionals that came to their skepticism because of my writing, an irony that fascinates me.

My belief is consistent with the much more famous beliefs of Dr. John Sarno. I don’t necessarily want to pride myself on being in agreement with Sarno, because he clearly got a lot wrong, and in particular he had cringe-inducingly poor understanding of trigger points. But, in this case, for whatever it’s worth (throwing a bone to the Sarno fans reading this), I am on the same page as Sarno.

How could so much pain be primarily caused by “just muscle”? Why would muscles and other tissues get into such an appalling state in the first place? If the back isn’t otherwise fragile, why is the muscle tissue? And why the back in particular?

All of these questions and many more need some kind of answers if we are to take trigger point therapy seriously.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

There is nothing “just” about muscle

The wisest back pain experts all freely admit that we simply don’t know what causes most back pain, and they aren’t easily impressed by any attempt fill in that blank. Nevertheless, I am going to take a stab at it. One of the contenders is just cranky muscle. It could be a major missing piece of the clinical puzzle of “non-specific” back pain.115 This is not such a strange idea — although we are leaving the trunk of the tree of knowledge, we’re not going all that far out on a limb either. Remember, I am a quackery-hating curmudgeon, and I am not going to carelessly promote a pseudoscientific idea!

The first thing to understand is that muscle can really hurt. It can be a source of misery, and we know this from examples like the pain caused by an acute cramp, the more insidious and slow-motion agony of neuropathic spasticity, and especially the chilling example of the “MS hug.”116 And although we "know" these things — nothing controversial about the MS hug that I know of — it’s worth noting that the symptoms of the MS hug are often dismissed or misinterpreted. Because medicine is a mess.

For every clinically obvious example like that, subtler ones are likely legion.

Even patients themselves are often amazed to discover the sensitivity of their own muscle tissue. They feel the pain, but they have no idea how vulnerable the tissue feels until it’s prodded. Even moderate pressure can cause strong discomfort. Back in the day, my massage clients often asked me, “What is that?” (Or they just said a bad word.) They could hardly believe my “just muscle” answer. (And, no, I wasn't being brutal. I was never that kind of massage therapist.) They assume that such sensitivity must come from some more delicate anatomy. Plenty of muscle tissue does not hurt when you poke it, even quite vigorously — presumably because it’s healthier in some way. But some muscle, some of the time, sure is sensitive! Presumably because is un-healthy in some way.117

What’s going on? Why would mere muscle ever hurt so much?

Most of us take muscle for granted. We think it is relatively simple, dumb stuff. Nope! Muscle contains multitudes and miracles. It is one of evolution’s craziest inventions.

It is never “just” or “mere” muscle: it is a breathtakingly complex, sensitive, and volatile tissue.119 It’s got a complex, difficult, and unrelenting job to do, which it achieves through dazzling complexity and coordination of organic chemistry. And its behaviour is highly sensitive to our mental state.

All complicated systems can fail in complicated and subtle ways. Sometimes muscles behaves oddly, or poorly, or gets “sick,” and the pain can be agonizing. Dr. John Sarno writes: “ … it is highly unlikely that a structural derangement could produce pain equal in severity to acute muscle spasm [by which he meant trigger points].”120 I second that. I’ve both experienced and witnessed what I sincerely believed to be good examples of absurdly painful muscle.

That said, many professionals just flat out do not believe in this — many out of frank ignorance, some due to informed and reasonable skepticism, and some lost between those extremes. So let's dig a little deeper into the idea.

What if there are rather a lot of sore spots?

“Myofascial pain syndrome” (MPS) was first studied by Drs. Janet Travell and David Simons in the 70s and 80s.121 In their notoriously incomplete and speculative explanation of MPS, physical and/or emotional stresses trigger a vicious cycle, and a trigger is basically a tiny cramp or spasm. A patch of muscle tissue clenches, choking off its own blood supply, resulting in oxygen and nutrient deprivation and stagnant tissue fluids that irritate sensory nerves and perpetuate the cycle.122 Some muscles seem to be more vulnerable to this phenomenon than others — the back muscles most of all. An updated version of this explanation, the “integrated hypothesis,” was spelled out in 2004 by Gerwin et al.,123 and is now the most widely known working theory.

For the purposes of this book, that working theory is what I will work with — with the understanding that it’s just a working theory, it has critics, and it could be wrong. Indeed, it almost certainly is at least partially wrong! Which Gerwin et al. have acknowledged.124 And which I also freely acknowledge, and even emphasize and explore at great length elsewhere.125

At great length. All of this is just highlights from my entire book about muscle pain, which is even larger than this one.

Only the explanation is controversial

Importantly, it’s only the explanation for sore patches of muscle tissue that is controversial. No one doubts that the sensitivity itself exists. The experts just argue about the why and the how and the what to do about it. Indeed, there’s a long, colourful history of arguing about this stuff: many people have tried to label/explain these mysterious patches of icky, achy muscle. The scientific literature was thick with theories long before the ideas of Travell and Simons started to dominate, and there are still several extant competing theories. A small sample:

  • In his best-selling book Mind Over Back Pain (and in his more recent Healing Back Pain), Sarno described the same problem and calls it “tension myositis syndrome,” attributing it mainly to stress-induced constriction of blood vessels causing oxygen starvation.126
  • Dr. C. Chan Gunn of Vancouver calls it “neuropathic pain,” and suggests another explanation: under-stimulated muscle may become extremely sensitive.127 Dr. Gunn’s ideas are also at the heart of one of the most popular and controversial treatment methods, “dry needling”: stabbing trigger points with acupuncture needles, basically! If that sounds painful, you’re right — it can be agonizing.
  • Quintner, Cohen, and Bove proposed that the pain is caused by inflamed nerve fibres, and/or “referred pain and tenderness” from deeper tissues with unspecified troubles and/or “altered central nociceptive mechanisms.”128

Not all of these ideas can possibly be the whole truth. No doubt it’s a classic case of the blind men trying to identify different parts of an elephant. For the purposes of this book, I will work with expanded “expanded integrated hypothesis,” based generally on my opinion that is probably the "least wrong" of all the options.129

Diagram of trigger point referral, showing a spot in the middle of the right side lumbar paraspinals, radiating pain mainly into the buttock, but also a little into the upper hamstrings.

Trigger point referral

Discomfort from low back trigger points point spreads in somewhat predictable patterns into the buttocks & legs & occasionally around to the side. This trigger point can cause pain all the way down to the knee — sensation that can get mistaken for “sciatica.”

Here’s what can be said with some confidence about the clinical phenomenon of trigger points

Trigger points are patches of muscle tissue (and perhaps other soft tissues) that are “unhealthy” in some way (maybe “micro cramps,” maybe something else). Some muscles seem to be more vulnerable than others — and seemingly the back muscles most of all, for unknown reasons.130 They are not only painful to touch, but are also closely associated with aching pain and stiffness that spreads out from an epicentre of sensitivity. “Referred pain” is a well-documented phenomenon, and occurs with most visceral pain to some degree; the pain originates in one location, but is felt in other locations (in surprisingly predictable patterns).

For example, people with low back pain routinely have trigger points that “send” pain into the buttocks and down the legs. These sensations can be extremely similar to pain produced by irritated nerves, like lumbar nerve roots, the sciatic nerve, or the cluneal nerves in the buttocks — often resulting in misdiagnosis.131132 In most people with low back pain, trigger points causing these radiating sensations can be located quite easily. Although the pressure is nowhere near a nerve,133 the spot is sensitive and radiates in a familiar way — clients say things like “it shoots down my legs” and “that’s exactly what my pain is like.”

Unsurprisingly, sometimes the “pinched nerve” pain goes away when these trigger points are treated. This is an example of how muscle might be the main cause of low back pain, even when no spinal injury has occurred — including the cases that scare people, the ones that seem unusually severe, strange, or “nervy.” Massage therapist Clair Davies (famous for writing a simple guide to trigger points), writes, “Back pain always has a myofascial component, no matter the official diagnosis. Although arthritis, bad discs, and displaced vertebrae come quickly to mind when your back hurts, back pain is very often nothing but referred pain from myofascial trigger points.”134 (Though not necessarily referred, and indeed often not referred — just muscle tissue aching right where the trigger points live.)

Or consider the opposite scenario, in which we begin with a genuine injury or mechanical problem: despite the presence of a painful factor other than muscle, it may soon be the least of your worries as trigger points crop up and begin to overshadow the original problem, causing pain that is even worse and more stubborn.135 This may be a typical course of events.

It is the nature of trigger points to both complicate other kinds of back pain and cause more of it. Hypothetically, muscles full of knots aren't as functional — not as good at their jobs. If true, that would likely boost vulnerability to injury, re-injury. Pain itself is also probably a risk factor for more hurting in the future,136 and that might be more true of trigger point pain just because it tends to be so persistent: the more it drags on, the more likely it is to haunt people in the future, like a traumatic memory.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The mind game in back pain: an overview of the psychology of back pain

This book is in the midst of extensive upgrades on the theme of the psychology of back pain (2023):

BEFORE — The book more or less embraced Sarno’s idea that back pain may often be “the new ulcer,” with some caveats, and aimed to persuade readers that one of the most important things any back patient can do is increase their confidence and optimism to achieve a “rational placebo” derived mainly from knowledge and (especially) “pain neuroscience education.” Although the importance stress relief was also strongly emphasized, the details of options like mindfulness and meditation, relaxation, and breathing exercises were all neglected, and mainstream psychology (mainly cognitive behavioural therapy) was completely missing.

AFTER: A more agnostic and modern perspective, with full reviews of all popular and mainstream ideas about the role of the mind in back pain, especially mindfulness and cognitive behavioural therapy. There will significantly less stress on stress, and somewhat less on PNE and the hope of top-down modulation as a major mechanism of relief. I do still think the "confidence cure" is an important and useful concept, but it will no longer be front and centre.

I am actively working on this transition into 2023. Some legacy content will remain for a while yet. I have already been working on this for months. I have a lot of background material prepared, and here’s what I’ve actually added to the book so far:

  • There’s a new high quality chapter about mainstream psychotherapy (so mainly cognitive behavioural therapy).
  • A full report on the study of pain reprocessing therapy that made headlines in the winter of 2022.
  • Yoga for back pain is now thoroughly explored. For a long time I only had a chapter explaining why it’s “not for everyone.” True! But not really the whole story. 🙂
  • This section!

Coming in time:

  • Malingering and psychosomatic pain. (I haven't started writing on this yet, but it’s been on my mind routinely for over a year now. I am mentally prepared for it!)
  • More about meditation/mindfulness, and especially the neurophysiology of relaxation and deep breathing exercises (drafts well underway).
  • Vagus nerve “resetting” and polyvagal theory (mostly written, just need to adapt and transplant).
  • Putting Sarno in his place a bit (but also still working with some of his better ideas).
  • And more! It’s a ginormous theme.

The big psychological picture

Chronic pain in general is often hard to attribute to any one factor, and back pain is the ultimate example of this, such a “perfect storm” of many possible factors that it’s almost inconceivable that the mind has no role to play. Even when back pain originates in frank pathology or trauma — which certainly happens to some people (I am married to one) — it will eventually be complicated by psychology in some way. Probably.

But how? Almost every big idea about the role of psychology in back pain is on thin scientific ice — both alternative and mainstream.

On the mainstream side, cognitive behavioural therapy is highly over-rated, and everyone is appalled by the ubiquitous all-in-your-head vibe emanating from most doctors like hard radiation (even when disavowed), and experts cannot even agree on the existence of psychosomatic pain (yes, that concept remains amazingly unclear scientifically).

On the alternative side, the power of the mind over pain has been a fountain of quackery for a century! There are undoubtedly some good ideas in that mess, but trying to find them is like going to the landfill to search for some good drapes.

There are immense problems with all of the most influential ideas in mind-body medicine, and most of the major personalities are controversial. Sarno, arguably the most influential expert on the topic of the psychology of back pain specifically, was certainly full of good and compassionate ideas, and he was good at expressing them … but he was also obviously drunk on his own Kool-AID by the end of his career, pumping out books about the power of the mind to heal practically anything. We all know what the road to Hell is paved with.

Key points and highlights in the psychology of back pain

  • Malingering — consciously pretending to have pain — probably does exist, but is fantastically rare compared to actual back pain, and not relevant to most patients or professionals reading this book. It is a consideration with back pain more than most other problems, however, because it is so often tangled up with legal battles about disability and injury. It’s also just an interesting sub-topic in its own right, so someday I may write more about it here.137
  • Psychosomatic back pain might exist, but no one’s really sure about that, and it’s probably rarely “pure” or “all” in your head. It’s more likely that the mind can exaggerate seeds of truth about pain — minor injuries and pathologies — rather than gin it up from scratch. And there are plenty of such potential seeds in the spine! And yet even this (psychological pain amplification) is controversial and may not actually exist.
  • Knowledge is power? An important neuropsychological perspective is that “pain is an opinion” generated by brains, a volatile perception based just as much on high level threat assessment as tissue insult. The pain “system” is an elaborate alarm system that leans towards better-safe-than sorry false alarms, with psychology as a major component. Spines in particular may be overprotected, which might partially explain the prevalence of human back pain — and reassurance, confidence, education, and an understanding of this principle (“Explain Pain” or “pain neuroscience education”) might all be helpful. But this is all hypothetical so far, with only scraps of direct evidence to support it.
  • “Coping” rather than curing is never truly satisfying to any back pain patient, but nevertheless it is probably the only solid ground in this topic… and effective coping might actually support genuine recovery. No one wants a consolation prize, but it’s better than no prize at all.
  • Lifestyle medicine — the pursuit of overall health, fitness, and happiness — is probably relevant to back pain by reducing biological vulnerability… and that in turn may call for psychological growth, maybe even a major mental makeover. The physiology of chronic anxiety, stress, depression and exhaustion is impressively toxic stuff, with loose but clear links to back pain, along with the social ills that obviously underly many of those problems — and hence the potential importance of a “biopsychosocial” perspective on back pain that includes essentially everything about people. Examples as simple as taking the edge off insomnia or blunting loneliness could be profoundly important to many chronic pain patients.
  • Complex pharmacotherapy with psychological goals — anxiolytics and anti-depressants — has shown some limited but clear potential to help back pain patients.
  • Cannabis (THC) is not usually thought of primarily as a psychotherapeutic treatment, but that might be how we should think about it: to the extent that it helps anyone with pain, its psychoactive effects are the most plausible active ingredient, rather than direct analgesia, which is does not appear to be capable of (although with cannabis there's a huge “it’s complicated” factor). See also things like ketamine and mushrooms and other mind-blowing substances that, while highly unpredictable, are notorious for their ability to radically shift mental state with occasionally impressive pain-relieving results.
  • Yoga and meditation/mindfulness are “acquired tastes” that are aesthetically and philosophically objectionable to many people, and yet oppressively obligatory treatment options. For those that want to pursue them, they likely have some value — if you can separate them from the nonsense (especially the toxic positivity that is one of the primary outputs of the Wellness Industrial Complex, and the tragically mistaken belief that most back pain is caused by a weak/wonky “core”).
  • Cognitive behavioural therapy is the 800-lb gorilla of modern psychology, over-hyped and over-rated, with shockingly weak evidence of efficacy for back pain. CBT is also the foundation of Smartphone apps like Curable, Pathways, and Ouchie — wildly popular and probably not useless, but also probably not revolutionary.
  • There are several major branded mind-body medicine approaches to chronic pain, but the best known one focused on back pain is Sarno’s insightful and compassionate but flawed “mind over back pain,” which basically boils down to blaming back pain on repressed emotions and (more specifically) “avoidance of psychic conflict.” It’s probably not all wrong, but it’s pretty thin ice scientifically. Pain reprocessing therapy is another notable example, especially because of the notoriously positive 2022 trial.

Back pain is steeped in more ideas about the role of the mind than any other single common condition. However, the topic is even bigger than that! There are many more ideas about the psychology of pain that aren’t relevant or important enough to include here. For a more general tour of the theme, see Mind Over Pain.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Is low back pain “the new ulcer”? Back pain as embodied stress/anxiety/depression

Back pain might be how some bodies say “no!” It could be the painful manifestation of an urgent desire and need for change — a reaction to stress. That turn of phrase is borrowed from the title of a book about stress-induced illness by Vancouver physician Gabor Maté.138

Back pain could also be how “the body keeps the score,” another phrase borrowed from a similar book.139

Let me stop the groaning: I know everyone is tired of hearing about the evils of stress, and the last thing anyone with chronic back pain needs is another reason to wonder if you haven’t somehow brought it on yourself. Low back pain is definitely not a basket-case diagnosis. Neither is back pain that is dominated by muscle. I am not telling you that having back pain means that you are a hypochondriac or that it’s “all in your head,” and I do not recommend meditation or yoga: those are tired clichés that miss the point.

Pure psychosomatic pain is probably as rare and exotic as hallucination (indeed, there are serious scientific questions about whether it’s even possible). While pain may be strongly affected by psychology, even disastrously amplified and prolonged, it is probably rarely “caused” by it. There isn’t even a pain-specific mental disorder defined for psychiatrists — there used to be, but it was eliminated because it was so problematic. There are so many hard-to-diagnose pathological causes of pain (an unfair share of which are in the back) that the experts judged there was too high a risk of prematurely diagnosing people with a mental disorder when there’s something actually wrong with them.140

The point here is that most pain begins with a seed of biological truth, some kind of injury or pathology, something that is objectively detectable in principle — but later it probably becomes increasingly sensitive to mental states to varying degrees and via various indirect mechanisms, much like high blood pressure, heart disease, asthma, or irritable bowel syndrome.141 Back pain is vulnerable to mental state.

The excessive sore spots of myofascial pain syndrome seem to disproportionately affected. Stress may be both a root cause and a complicating factor in MPS, much as MPS is both a root cause of some back pain and complication of nearly other types of back pain.142 Although it is under-researched, many studies have shown links between stress and low back pain.143144145146 The likely potency of placebo in the treatment of back pain is also telling: nothing works magic on a stress-aggravated condition like optimism!

The boiling frog problem

People often do not realize that they are suffering from enough stress to cause trouble. They have clear biological signs of stress — and this is an important premise for what’s coming here. The links between personality type and stress are a little murky and complicated, but they are certainly there:147

Your style, your temperament, your personality have much to do with whether you regularly perceive opportunities for control or safety signals when they are there, whether you consistently interpret ambiguous circumstances as implying good news or bad, whether you typically seek out and take advantage of social support. Some folks are good at modulating stress in these ways, and others are terrible.

Robert M Sapolsky, Why Zebras Don’t Get Ulcers, 2004, p. 309.

There are two major personality types that not only involve a lot of stress, but some degree of obliviousness to it:

  1. The stereotype of the “Type A” personality with its aggressive coping mechanisms, by eagerly tackling fears directly… and by seeing threats where there are none, just in case, which is why it becomes a problem. These people are likely to make a virtue of stress, re-framing it as though it were a good thing, eager to believe that “stress is your friend” (it’s not).148
  2. The stoic, repressive personality — people who feel and act calm and methodical, and genuinely believe that they are, but they are actually conforming intensely to social norms, and controlling and organizing their lives to the point of suffocation. Dr. Robert Sapolsky: “It can be enormously stressful to construct a world without stressors.”149 Sarno: “one can be stressed without feeling tension” and “there are chronic daily stresses in people’s lives that are more insidious and more harmful [than crises] in their long-term biological consequences. Internally generated stresses take their toll without in any way seeming out of the ordinary150 (emphasis mine).

And then there’s just straight-up avoidance, acting stoic because stress is the same as anxiety, and anxiety is stigmatized and many people want to hide it. It is socially risky to express anxiety — no one wants to be perceived as jittery! It can even be verboten as a self-judgement as well; some people perform stoicism for their own sake, trying to fake it until they make it.

Sometimes the source of tension is not obvious … I recall a young married woman who … denied being tense or nervous … she was known to be a very jolly, easygoing person. Only after a long discussion did she reveal that her strategy for coping with life’s problems was to put them out of her mind. This is a foolproof formula for generating tension. Putting things out of one’s mind doesn’t get rid of them …

John Sarno, Mind Over Back Pain, p53

Ulcers were a socially acceptable symptom of stress for a long time. Rather than being a nervous individual, you could be a hardass with an ulcer. But now everyone “knows” that ulcers are stress-induced, whether they really are or not, so ulcers are much less effective as a disguise for emotional stress — and maybe this might partially explain why ulcers have been declining since the 1970s.151 (Aren’t ulcers caused by an infection? Yes, but the devil is in the details, and the details are in this footnote:152 )

Instead, Sarno proposed, North Americans increasingly make back pain into a perfect new “hiding place” for nervous tension: a path of least resistance more than a choice. Because of the widespread belief that backs are fragile, people with back pain are readily seen as victims of biology, instead of stressed out basket cases. Perhaps in time, if the stress-mediated nature of back pain becomes a widespread idea, it won’t be as useful in this way.

Psychosomatic disorders are physical symptoms that mask emotional distress. The very nature of the physical presentation of the symptoms hides the distress at its root, so it is natural that those affected will automatically seek a medical disease to explain their suffering.

It's All in Your Head, by Suzanne O’Sullivan, 8

The vicious cycle hypothesis

Once an episode of back pain starts, pain itself, immobility, and the fear of more pain become nasty new sources of stress. Thus, back pain may “cause itself.”153 Pain, fear, and immobility constitute an axis of evil, each reinforcing the other.

Tragically, medical care for back pain often spooks patients, making the vicious cycle even more vicious:

The system generates great fear. I recall a woman who became terrified when she read in a well-known book on the back that if you had a mildly herniated disc and became active too soon it would fully herniate and then require surgery. Another patient was told by the doctor as she left the hospital after refusing surgery, “You will be back in two weeks screaming in pain and begging me for an operation.”

Dr. John Sarno, Mind Over Back Pain, p105

A leading cause of back pain may be health care itself,154 in part accounting for the increased frequency of low back pain in modern times and in industrialized nations. More generally, the fear is stimulated by the many common myths and misconceptions about the fragility of spines — which are perpetuated by nearly everyone, not just doctors.

Pain, fear & immobility constitute an “axis of evil,” each reinforcing the other & boosting the risk that the pain will drag on.

That’s a nice clear picture, but we can boil it down to something even simpler …

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Pain and fear, together at last: an even simpler vicious cycle

The diagram above — the pain-fear-immobility cycle — is one of many similar ways of visualizing what happens in chronic low back pain. For dramatic effect, I’ve cast immobility and muscle dysfunction as the villains: very specific and physical parts of the vicious cycle. Immobility is a problem behaviour, for instance: something you do (or don’t do). That may not be quite right, but describing it in this way is no accident: I want to make it real. Something that implies action. Be more mobile! Keep muscles happy!

Immobilization is indeed a common way to show fear, and it’s much easier to work with the consequences of fear than fear itself. And it may well be a particularly important factor in muscle pain.

And yet fear itself is actually all we need to keep pain going in that diagram. Immobility is redundant. You don’t need that particular behavioural piece of the vicious puzzle to explain the stubbornness of back pain. You can take immobility out of the picture, and the vicious cycle carries on, powered by fear and pain alone.

We tend to assume that the persistence of pain means that there is damage and danger, assumptions that are in themselves direct causes of pain. If your brain thinks that the situation is injurious and dangerous, it has the ability to make sure it feels that way — regardless of whether or not you actually behave fearfully by immobilizing yourself. (See Woolf155 — a particularly important reference.)

Bear with me for a moment for an amusing example. (This is a bit of a reach, but it’s worth it to get a chuckle out of this difficult subject.) There’s an episode of The IT Crowd that has fun with massage: “Something Happened” (Season 4 Episode 3). Roy has terrible back pain, but he doesn’t particularly want a massage:

I’ve never enjoyed having massages. I don’t like being naked in front of strangers, and I can never relax if I think someone might play Norah Jones.

Roy, The IT Crowd, “Something Happened” (Season 4 Episode 3)

Nevertheless, he gives it a try and he’s actually kind of digging it, until the “something” happens: his therapist concludes the otherwise professional treatment with a quick kiss on the buttock! (News bulletin: this does not actually happen. This is amusing, fanciful satire.)

Yes, if you watch Game of Thrones, you have seen that massage therapist before. More seriously (and yet still funny!) is how Roy describes the results of his massage:

Roy attributes the failure to the extreme tension caused by a violating butt kiss — as one does — but there’s a deeper truth here: after a massage that seems to provide great relief at the time, many people do indeed soon end up right back where they started. Despite the nice clichés about feeling relaxed after massage, patients can actually get off the table feeling surprisingly unpleasant, wrung out in a not-so-good way: fragile, vulnerable, and tense. In Roy’s case, it was the kiss on the butt that did it! In real life, therapists of all kinds often unwittingly scare their patients with careless talk of what’s wrong in there, how serious it is, how they need more therapy, and so on, which can leave patients feeling more worried and anxious about their backs than they need to be. And that, in turn, is more than just a side effect: it actually perpetuates the problem.

It cannot be said enough: this is not an “all in your head” message. It’s an “all in your brain” message — not psychology, but neurology. Or, as Dr. Lorimer Moseley puts it, “Pain really is in the mind, but not in the way you think.”156 Here’s his description of this more purely neurological vicious cycle:

This is where our understanding of pain itself becomes part of a vicious cycle. We know that as pain persists the nociception [danger signalling] system becomes more sensitive. What this means is that the spinal cord sends danger messages to the brain at a rate that overestimates the true danger level. This is a normal adaption to persistent firing of spinal nociceptors. Because pain is (wrongly) interpreted to be a measure of tissue damage, the brain has no option but to presume that the tissues are becoming more damaged. So when pain persists, we automatically assume that tissue damage persists.

I think it goes like this: “more pain = more damage = more danger = more pain” and so on and so forth.

All of this strongly suggests that anything that reduces fear might be therapeutic. Reducing the fear part of the equation — not just empty reassurance, but good reasons for genuine, unforced confidence — is a recurring theme throughout the rest of this book. Refusing to be immobilized is probably one of the best specific strategies for rebuilding confidence (which is why I gave it special focus in the pain-fear-immobilization diagram). The next section is another great example …

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Chronic low back pain is not so chronic: the pseudo-myth of chronicity

Famously, most back pain doesn’t last long: most people get better fairly soon. Specifically, at least half of people with back pain will improve or even recover completely within just a month.157 But then what?

What happens when back pain doesn’t go away within a few weeks? When does acute pain become chronic… and how long does chronic low back pain last? The duration that conventionally distinguishes “acute” from “chronic” cases is 6-12 weeks (depends who you ask). It’s an arbitrary number, not a data-based one — and it comes with the insidious, confidence-nuking insinuation that those who do not recover by that deadline are doomed to a much longer battle with chronic pain.

You do not disastrously become an impossible case the moment you hit week thirteen. In fact, if you’ve had chronic low back pain for less than a year, I’ve got great news for you: your ordeal may soon be over, no matter what you do about it. Or don’t do about it. There is good evidence about this, which this article will explore in detail.158159

Obviously low back pain can last for many years, sometimes constant, and other times coming and going (chronic episodic back pain). But these are a worst-case scenarios, and here’s the good news is that chronic low back pain is:

  • chronic low back pain is less common than people fear
  • chronic low back pain is less severe than people fear
  • chronic low back pain is less chronic than people fear

The pseudo-myth of back pain chronicity

The pseudo-myth is that chronic back pain is common, severe, and rarely ever lets up. It’s not quite a full-strength myth for two reasons:

  1. Chronic back pain is real and serious. The pseudo-myth is an exaggeration of the truth, not total departure from it.
  2. No one is really out there “promoting” the myth, or erroneously defending it. You’re not likely to catch a doctor actually saying that you’re doomed after twelve weeks of back pain — but it might be implied.

Back pain is generally over-treated and over-medicalized with lots of medical drama with minimal benefit for patients (exhibit A: the infamous overprescription of imaging, too often and too soon). Part of that drama is a pervasive attitude of defeatism and alarmism about the prognosis of chronic low back pain. Most healthcare professionals know enough to reassure their patients that a new episode of back pain is likely to resolve quickly, but many of those same professionals simply postpone their pessimism and alarmism, defeating the purpose of their reassurances, and often undermining patient confidence long before chronicity actually arrives.

It is not at all reassuring to be told that “most cases go away with two or three months” if it’s accompanied by the strong implication that “you’re screwed if it doesn’t!” Patients given these conflicting messages are likely to start worrying that their pain will might not go away in time to avoid the spectre of chronicity.

Diagram showing acute pain giving way to chronic around the uncertain border of 12 weeks.

There is no clear transition from acute to chronic pain, but pain that persists beyond 9–12 weeks is usually deemed chronic. The surprising thing? Most chronic low back pain doesn’t stay “orange” for a whole year.

Good news: credible evidence that plenty of chronic back pain backs off!

If you’ve had chronic low back pain for less than a year, I’ve got great news for you: your ordeal may soon be over. A good Australian study by showed that “prognosis is moderately optimistic for patients with chronic low back pain.”

“Many studies provide good evidence for the prognosis of acute low back pain,” the authors explain. “Relatively few provide good evidence for the prognosis of chronic low back pain.”

And the prognosis is better than most people would expect.

This research is different from other studies of chronic low back pain, which often focus on patients who already have a well-established track record of long-term problems: in other words, the people who had already drawn the short straw before they were selected for study, and are therefore much more likely to carry right on feeling rotten. But what if you studied fairly new cases of chronic low back pain? How many of them fade away, and how many of them drag on?

And isn’t that what low back pain patient most want to know? Especially after suffering for 3-4 months? Just how chronic is chronic back pain? No so chronic after all…

More than a third of chronic cases recover within a year — which isn’t very chronic

Costa et al followed patients who had not yet recovered from their new cases of chronic low back pain, and found that “more than one third” recovered within nine more months. That’s a quite a happy number.

Yes, of course, that does still leave almost two thirds of patients who do continue to suffer past the year-mark — and that’s not such a happy number. This is chronic low back pain we’re talking about here, after all — it’s not nothing! But the surprising and promising thing is that so many patients in this study — almost 40% — actually did recover by the one-year anniversary of their pain. These are people who didn’t get better in the first three months … and who would have been told by many doctors that they were officially “chronic” at that point.

This evidence is a great foundation for more substantive and lasting optimism about back pain. But wait, there’s more!

How did it go for 11,000 more people with chronic low back pain?

Sure, it’s great that a third of people recover entirely, but there’s still something important missing here: what happens to everyone else? Do they improve? Or do they get stuck with just as much back pain at a year as they had at three months? Obviously that does happen to a few unlucky souls.

But definitely not most people.

There’s surprisingly little hard evidence on the prognosis of chronic low back pain: as of 2023, there’s still just one other good study … and it also came from Costa et al. Busy researchers!

In 2012, they pooled a whole bunch of data about the progression of both acute and chronic back pain, generally validating their 2009 finding that chronic pain improves, and some of it resolves — further debunking the idea that chronic is permanent. Yes, lots of people still have significant pain and disability a year after being stuck with the “chronic” label… but not everyone, and almost everyone improves quite a bit. Costa et al. crunched the numbers on over 11,000 patients in 33 different studies of back pain patients, and split them up into acute and chronic (pain for more/less than 12 weeks). Here’s how pain in those groups changed in the year after their trouble started:

Progression of acute vs chronic back pain (Costa et al)
acute chronic
onset 69/100 60–80?160
6 weeks 28 55
26 weeks 12 29
1 year 4 17
Chart of key data from Costa et al. 2012.

Whether pain was acute or chronic to begin with, pain declines in very similar way over a year. Although chronic is clearly worse than acute on average, it also declines slowly & significantly over the months — a striking contrast to the pattern that people fear about back pain chronicity.

Risk factors: who did well or poorly in the Costa trial, and why?

Did the people who didn’t recover in Costa et al’s 2009 trial have anything in common? The study also looked at risk factors, and found some patterns. The patients whose pain just kept going were those who had worse pain, more disability, and more fear (“perceived risk of persistent pain”) — no surprise there. They were also the patients with a history of previous sick leave — not for back pain, but for other things, people who may be generally unwell.

A little more surprising was that they had less education: better educated people recovered better.

And (my favourite) the patients with persistent pain also tended to be non-Australian. That’s right: native Australians in Australia get less chronic back pain than non-Australians in Australia! Not sure what to do with that information — don’t move to Australia and get low back pain, I guess? Sound medical advice.

Who does poorly with low back pain? Known risk factors for chronicity

Back pain chronicity is impossible to predict reliably, but there are some signs to be alert for. Although very complex, the oversimplified truth is so obvious it almost feels silly to say it: the worst and most intimidating cases of back pain are the most likely to last! But we can get more specific…

  1. Anxiety and pessimism. Obviously more severe back pain tends to be more emotionally challenging, but there are other critical factors too, like how the pain menaces work and fun. (It is not clear whether anxiety and pessimism are a cause of chronicity, or merely a natural consequence of the severity that leads to chronic back pain. More on this below.)
  2. Leg pain, pain in the neck and shoulders, and difficulty walking or getting dressed.
  3. No position or movement eases the pain. This can change with time, so the more and sooner you can see relief with some movement or position, the better. This is the phenomenon of “centralization” — relief in response to specific repeated movements or sustained postures. We see this in roughly 40% of people with back pain, and it is quite strongly linked to better outcomes.161 It’s possible, maybe even likely, that centralization is just a sign of a milder case of back pain,162 but I’m including it because it’s such a famous signal. (See the centralization chapter for more.)

Back pain chronicity risk quiz

The STarT back pain questionnaire is a nine-question quiz that does a good job of identifying low, medium, and high risk of a poor outcomes for people with back pain.163 The full questionnaire is intended to be used by clinicians with patients, but it can be understood and used by anyone, and there’s an online calculator.

But scoring the official version is a bit tortuous (there’s literally a flow-chart for it, and worse164) — so I think the world could use a simpler version. This is my own slightly simplified self-serve quiz for patients. While the full quiz is scientifically validated, and that matters, it’s clearly not an exact science, and simplified version is just fine for getting a rough sense of your risk level.

The more of these statements apply to you, the higher your risk of chronicity, and the more likely you are to benefit from skilled and personalized care. Most people scoring three or less probably need not professional help at all, as long as the situation doesn’t get worse. If you score low initially, but then much higher three weeks later, you should seek out help.

    In last couple weeks…

  • My back pain has spread down into one or both legs.
  • I have had some pain in the shoulders or neck.
  • I am walking less and/or I’m dressing more slowly.
  • I don’t think it’s safe to be physically active.
  • I am worrying a lot about this back pain.
  • I am struggling to enjoy things I normally enjoy.
  • This back pain has been extremely bothersome.

The relationship of back pain and anxiety and/or depression

This sub-section probably contradicts other parts of the book! My views on this topic are evolving, and a large revision of the content on psychology is (still) underway in 2023.

Knowing that your chronic back pain probably won’t be as chronic as you fear is inherently valuable. But could that kind of reassurance actually help? And does being afraid of a bad outcome actually make it more likely?

Anxiety, pessimism, and depression might cause exactly what we fear — a self-fulfilling prophecy. And reassurance might actually scare the back pain away and, all other things being equal, produce a better prognosis than if you were freaked out.

That’s a very tempting idea, but No one actually knows that anxiety causes or exacerbates back pain, or vice versa, because the right evidence simply does not exist (despite some strong expert opinions to the contrary in both directions). All anyone can do is speculate. All we do know is that there is a some kind of relationship,165 and that there are indeed psychological risk factors for back pain … but correlations and risk factors are not “causes.”

It is plausible that anxiety and depression have can make back pain worse (at least). And if that’s the case, then reassurance and correction of the misinformation might be very valuable.166 Here’s a hypothetical case study to illustrate how all this might work…

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

A trigger point checklist: does this sound like you?

There are several classic characteristic signs and symptoms of back pain that might be caused mainly by cranky muscle, either from the start, or as a persistent complication of an original problem that has slowly faded away. This section provides a quick checklist to give you a rough idea, plus a slow checklist with more detail.

For most people, most of the time, confirming a trigger point diagnosis is simple enough. Check all that apply — if you have more than half of these, and no other apparent explanation for your pain, you probably have a trigger point or two.

  • You have sore spots in muscles.
  • Your pain usually occurs in specific areas of your body.
  • The problem feels more like muscles than joints.
  • Your pain is primarily dull, aching, and nagging.
  • You feel a lot of stiffness as well as pain.
  • Affected areas feel weak and heavy.
  • Stretching is appealing (but not very effective).
  • Hot showers and baths are usually helpful.
  • Anti-inflammatory medications don’t really work.

And now for the slow checklist. This list is deliberately similar to the slow checklist for myofascial pain syndrome in the trigger points tutorial, but it is customized for back pain. Muscle pain in the back is a different critter than muscle pain in general — many distinctions are made throughout this section. Disclaimer: obviously this is not an exact science. Indeed, as some would argue, it’s not any kind of science, and they’re certainly right about this: this is all based on professional opinion and experience. We don’t know what trigger points actually are, just how they seem. And how they seem is bound to be unclear and overlap with other issues quite a bit. Any one checkmark in this list could be misleading, and even a whole bunch of them aren’t diagnostic, just “suggestive.” Check every item here, and what does it mean? The pain is more likely to be driven by muscle, but hardy guaranteed. You are bound to have some of these signs, regardless of the cause of your back pain. Roughly less than one third means that trigger points probably aren’t an important factor in your case. More than two thirds? There’s a decent change that trigger points are a significant factor.

  • Sensitive under pressure. By definition, trigger points are sensitive patches of soft tissue. If you have a trigger point problem in the back, then your back muscles may be more sensitive to pressure. That is, it will hurt to firmly poke your low back and upper gluteal muscles, compared to muscles in problem-free areas of your body. These are big muscles, so they may not be sensitive to light pressure — use a medium to firm pressure and check in several locations. And it probably is possible to have deep trigger points that cannot be provoked by poking.
  • Feels like muscle! Pain is a difficult sensation to interpret, but trigger points do often feel like a muscle problem in ways that may be hard to specify. Many small clues can contribute to this qualitative impression: a sense of moderate depth (deeper than skin, shallower than bone), for instance, or sensitivity to flexing and stretching. Muscle pain might be much sharper than you expect, but usually it is dull and aching. If your pain is mostly aching, and nagging, with a strong “stiffness” component — and there are no other kinds of pain, like stabbing or burning or electrical sensations — then muscle may be the source of your troubles.
  • No other obvious cause for pain! And by “obvious” I mostly mean “injury.” One of the simplest ways to diagnose trigger points is simply by elimination: if there is no obvious trauma, then trigger points are somewhat more likely. Only a significant and recent accident qualifies as a possible traumatic cause of your back pain: a high-velocity sports injury, falling off a ladder, or a muscle strain while lifting a sofa-bed. Backs are pretty tough — they cannot normally be damaged by subtleties like “twisting the wrong way” or “more golf than usual.”167 There are other reasons pain might persist after healing, but trigger points are a strong possibility.
  • You have back trauma, but it’s old. Perhaps you really did injure your back … once upon a time. However, backs heal like any other part of your body. If your injury happened more than twelve weeks ago, you have probably healed entirely or mostly, and any continuing pain that you have is therefore more likely to be caused by trigger points. (There are other reasons pain might persist after healing! But trigger points are a strong possibility.) This is the “out of the frying pan, into the fire” phenomenon, discussed in another section and in a separate article, Muscle Pain as an Injury Complication: The story of how I finally “miraculously” recovered from the pain of a serious shoulder injury, long after the injury itself had healed.
  • You crave stretch. You crave it because trigger points are strongly linked to sensations of stiffness. For contrast, it is not an appealing idea to stretch a damaged spine. The nervous system usually strongly warns you away from that! When you stretch an aching muscle, it may feel difficult or even as though it amplifies the symptoms — like you are “pulling on the pain” — but it probably also feels relieving, like scratching an itch. Related: muscle pain tends to make people generally squirmy, a craving for movement in general.
  • … but stretch usually fails. Although it seems like a good idea and often feels nice at the time, stretching trigger points usually fails to actually do much good. It’s not an effective treatment. Sometimes it might provide more relief, but most MPS sufferers will be underwhelmed by the results of stretching. However, because it does tend to take the edge off, stretch remains appealing.

    Unfortunately, the muscles typically affected in the low back are also biomechanically difficult to stretch very much,168 so it often feels futile, like trying to scratch an itch you can’t quite reach. Back fear can definitely be a factor here: stretch is also limited by the brain,169 so the anxious back pain patient may not crave stretch simply because of a freaked out nervous system.

  • Aching, not sharp. Your pain is primarily dull, aching, nagging pain, as opposed to sharp, stabbing, burning. Although nearly any quality of pain is possible, and flared up trigger points can occasionally get hotter and stabbier, most pain of this type is just an ache. There aren’t many other unclear causes of pain that ache like this. For instance, arthritis can have an aching quality, but it’s often sharper and hotter.
  • Similar pain in other areas. Trigger point pain is “patchy,” afflicting some predictable areas of the body, especially the meatier tissues of the trunk (neck, shoulders, upper back, low back, and hips). Different problems in those areas would probably cause different kinds of pain, or more widespread and uniform pain (like the diffuse all-over sensitivity of the flu, or muscle soreness after unfamiliar exertion, which always affects an entire muscle group uniformly). Trigger point pain usually flares up in no more than one or two regions of the body at a time. If you have one or two other similar pains in the other areas, it’s more likely that they are all muscle pain.
  • Abnormal texture. You might be able to feel a lump in your muscle and a hard and ropy texture around it, but then again you might not. This is an unreliable way to diagnose trigger points, but it’s particularly tricky in the back, because there are many normal anatomical structures that feel like bumps and ropy structures, as well as common harmless abnormal structures, especially “back mice” (lipomas, more about them below). However, in some muscles in the area, bumps do tend to stand out nicely.170 But I want to be very clear that lump-hunting in the back is mostly a wild goose chase. But it might reinforce other signs and symptoms, so it’s worth being aware of.
  • Anti-inflammatories don’t help much. Since there is little or no inflammation per se involved in muscle pain, anti-inflammatories like Aspirin or ibuprofen (Motrin, Advil, etc) mostly don’t help much. If they do help, that can mean that pain does have a more “traditional” inflammatory nature (associated with tissue damage, such as a wounded facet joint or a ruptured intervertebral disc). Some other conditions, notably fibromyalgia, also cause pain that doesn’t respond well to anti-inflammatories, so this sign is hardly a slam dunk on its own. Note that the effects of other drugs are not very informative.171
  • Wandering pain. A classic sign of muscle-dominated pain is symptoms that change location, either moving to another area of the body altogether, or erratically shifting around and spreading out from a predictable epicentre. In addition to moving, trigger point pain also often changes quality, and comes and goes without much rhyme or reason (the “outta nowhere” phenomenon). Not all trigger point pain is so erratic, but it often is. For contrast, pain location is more consistent when it comes from injury, arthritis, or common kinds of neuropathy. Trigger points in the low back may be more entrenched and consistent than other areas of the body. Nevertheless, the pain is usually still less consistent than injury pain.
  • Mirror image pain. Damaged anatomy is rarely neatly symmetrical. Trigger points, however, quite often have “evil twins” in the same location on the opposite side of the body. Many cases of muscle pain dominate a single side but will, occasionally, flip to the other side — and even a single instance of this is a near guarantee that injured tissue is not to blame, because injured tissues can’t change sides. So, if you do ever have symmetrical pain, it’s more likely to be caused by muscular trigger points than by injury. Also, it’s a common pattern with low back pain patients that their pain flips to the other side following some kind (any kind) of treatment.172
  • There are signs of chronic or severe stress in your life. If the connection between back pain and stress is not obvious in your case — flare ups of back pain during crises — there may be many other signs of it. People who suffer from excessive muscle pain often have a medical history that includes other consequences of stress and stress-sensitive conditions (e.g. ulcers, panic attacks, insomnia, irritable bowel syndrome, etc).173
  • A “numb” leg. Trigger points cause weakness in the affected muscles and a kind of numbness often mistaken for a nerve pinch — sciatica. But this numbness is a deep, dead, heavy feeling … not the more profound skin-numbness that you can get in specific areas when your low back nerve roots or sciatic nerve are in real trouble. This distinction is explained in more detail below.
  • Heat helps. Hot showers and baths and other forms of heating, even just warm weather, are almost always helpful. Most kinds of chronic pain are helped by heat to some degree (ha!)174, with fibromyalgia pain as the most interesting exception (usually worsened by hot weather).175 But patients with ordinary muscle pain almost always prefer heat. If you like to be toasty, score a point for a trigger point diagnosis.
  • Symptoms are aggravated by muscular effort and overexertion. Assuming that trigger points are some kind of muscle dysfunction, it follows that they might flare up when we demand more function from the muscle, and this is commonly reported. Muscles can be exhausted either by an intense exercise like shoveling, or by sneakier postural stress, like an awkward working posture. If you predictably have flare-ups of pain in situations where muscles are working, then muscle might well the problem. Back muscles seem especially prone to postural stresses — sustained positions like stooping over while gardening, cleaning a bathtub (but not just slouching lazily).
  • The pain of stuckness. The discomfort of trigger points may be experienced as stiffness following periods of immobilization (e.g. hours in an airplane seat, or even a movie). This is a weak signal, because there are several other possible causes of stiffness, particularly in older people (the progression of many subtle sources of inflammation that accumulate over the years, known as “inflammaging,” which manifests in many ways but most obviously as arthritis). Relatively widespread stiffness in a younger person with no other known health issues makes trigger points a more plausible suspect.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

“Outta nowhere”: seemingly random episodes of low back pain

Being an adult is mostly about being exhausted, wishing you hadn’t made plans, waiting to take your bra off, wondering how you can fall asleep and stay asleep, missing someone or something, becoming forgetful, and wondering how you hurt your back.

source unknown

“I just can’t figure out what sets it off!”

Seemingly random flare-ups are one of the signature symptoms of many kinds of chronic pain, but particularly low back pain, and probably any pain problem dominated by muscle. This time-bomb effect is probably mostly attributable to pre-existing painless vulnerabilities, which can be pushed over into a more serious pain problems by surprisingly minor changes. There are a bunch of ways to be vulnerable before you actually notice, some of which we have little control over, others more so.

For instance, systemic inflammation (immune activation) is probably quite volatile, and waxes and wanes for our whole lives in response to physiological challenges, insults, and variables, many of which we are oblivious too. These changes are just too cryptic and messy to easily link to flare-ups.

Sneaky sources of physiological overload — stressing tissue more than we realize — are a factor we might be able to learn and manage more easily. Tissue can get into some trouble before it becomes obvious to us (stress fractures are a classic example), and then one day just a bit more activity pushes us over the threshold of injury. However, that rarely seems like pain truly out of “nowhere.” A lot of back pain flares up with essentially zero apparent provocation … and often disappears just as strangely. For that mercurial weirdness, you need to explain why pain would come and go quite easily, without any apparent provocation.

Latent trigger points

“Latent” trigger points may be a common form of pre-existing painless vulnerability. Trigger points exist in two states: latent and active, or quiet and loud.

  1. Latent trigger points are present but painless … until provoked. Like a pimple that you don’t notice until pressure is applied to it.
  2. An active trigger point hurts no matter what, throbbing like a severe pimple that hurts constantly.

Latent trigger points may lurk unsuspected for weeks, months, or years, finally becoming active and painful with minor provocation — the straw that broke the camel’s back. This creates the illusion that the symptoms have come “outta nowhere,” in response to minor physical and emotional stresses, when in fact the problem was developing for a long time before you felt it.

The “outta nowhere” phenomenon is a familiar feature of many chronic pain problems, and perhaps that’s because of the role of trigger points and the way they easily pop in and out of latency, like submarines surfacing and diving again.

Morning back pain

One of the best common examples of “outta nowhere” back pain is waking up with it. This is a major sub-topic, and there's a (very large) chapter about it: Morning back pain.

Sensitization and fear

John Sarno wrote: “How can severe back pain be started by incidents so variable in intensity? … In my view they are all triggers and have nothing to do with the basic cause of pain.”

Erratic pain seems to occur more dramatically in the low back than other areas. “Sensitization” is another possible explanation for this.

Sensitization is a normal process that occurs with even the tiniest wound: the area around a cut rapidly gets sensitive to stimulation, which encourages us to baby it. For simple, minor trauma, that’s all there is to it: hurt things get more ouchy until they heal.

But that effect can get chronic and abnormally excessive, probably for both psychological and physiological resons.

If pain is an alarm, pathological sensitization is a neurological problem with false alarms: bigger reactions to smaller provocations. Pain will be a worse experience — more intense and/or more of an emotional tornado — if we are convinced it represents a more dangerous problem than we actually have. And back pain is intimidating! So sensitization is probably often a major factor, lowering the threshold at which we notice tissue problems, and exaggerating them when we do.

Sounds awful, but the good news is that sensitization might also be eased by a wide variety of factors: anything that affects our overall sense of safety and well-being. This remains unproven, but plausible based on what we do know.

And so it is probably one reason why back pain seems to surge and ease unpredictably. It also might be one of the reasons that past back pain is a major risk factor for future back pain. Sensitization may be in place as a vulnerability long before a flare-up of back pain occurs.

When back pain suddenly shows up, we are tempted to blame it on the last minor stressor that affected it, such as a soft bed in a hotel. This is like blaming your bankruptcy on the last latte you bought before your account finally went into the red.

Todd Hargrove, Playing With Movement: How to explore the many dimensions of physical health and performance, 2019 p. 95, ch. 4: Stress and Adaptation

Trigger points might be a sign of sensitization — perhaps those sore spots are one of the things that happens because we’re sensitized. Maybe they are sensitization.

Regardless of the specific mechanism of pain — trigger point, irritated joint, whatever — low back pain may be volatile because it part because it’s emotionally overbearing, which is a powerful driver of sensitization. The fear factor seems to be more pronounced with low back pain than any other common area of pain, for a variety of reasons.

Another idea: the intensity and rapid escalation may be due to the substantial mass of the musculature involved, combined with the intimidating nature of pain in that region. There’s a lot of muscle in the thighs too, but pain there isn’t as worrisome … and pain in the neck can be very worrisome, but there’s a lot less muscle. The back has both a lot of muscle and it’s a scarier place to hurt.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Morning back pain

“I woke up with it” is an amazingly common description of how low back pain started. (And neck pain, and headaches, and even more.176).

Why? Is sleeping dangerous? Probably not: waking up with back pain rarely indicates a serious problem. Both mornings and backs are unusually vulnerable to some common minor sources of pain.

This is mostly about chronic back pain that’s at its worst in the morning, a common pattern. Even many people who are more or less pain-free during the day may still experience routine and significant irritation and stiffness first thing in the morning. But attacks of acute back pain are also much more common in the first few hours of the day,177 and that phenomenon may be related to why chronic back pain tends to be bad in the morning.

Morning back pain is a tough problem to treat because most of it probably has several subtle chronic causes, but there may be some opportunities for treatment.

Photograph of an unmade bed, representing the relationship between sleep and back pain.

Is pain & stiffness your alarm clock? Do you bail out of bed early every morning with low back pain, neck pain & more?

The 6 big causes of morning back pain

  1. Inflammatory back pain: pathological inflammation from autoimmune disease, which is often serious, relatively rare, and fairly well known.
  2. “Inflammaging”: slow but steady increase in chronic mild inflammation as we age.
  3. Myofascial pain syndrome and/or fibromyalgia: back pain is often the hot spot in the body for muscle pain and otherwise unexplained widespread aches and pains, and there are some reasons why these conditions may be worse in the morning, but they are mysterious and controversial.
  4. Insomnia and poor quality sleep is, of course, the most basic explanation for morning back pain. A good night of sleep is an effective painkiller, and a lousy one is the opposite.
  5. Osteomalacia (vitamin D deficiency): vitamin D deficiency is probably quite common, is linked to chronic pain, and morning bone aching in particular.
  6. Awkward sleeping postures: although this is the most “obvious” reason for waking up with a kink in your back, it might be worse than you expect.

3 myths about the causes of morning back pain

There are some popular ideas about what causes back pain that are probably wrong (or at least a lot more rare and trivial than believed):

  1. Bad mattresses are generally over-rated as a factor in back pain.
  2. Psychological factors are also often given too much weight. The mind is relevant to a lot of chronic pain, but morning back pain is particularly unlikely to be psychosomatic.
  3. Intervertebral disc swelling is a popular specific scapegoat, but it’s highly speculative and quite likely wrong.

All of these causes are discussed below, along with many treatment options and suggestions. Click the green headings above to jump down to a section.

The scariest type of morning back pain: inflammatory back pain

The closest thing to back pain that is truly prominent in the morning is inflammatory back pain (IBP), or spondyloarthritis.178 Although IBP is well known to medical science, it often eludes diagnosis, its biology is mysterious, and morning symptoms specifically are a stumper, as unexplained as joints that ache before a storm. It’s just something IBP does.

But don’t panic! Although studies have confirmed that morning is a common time for inflammatory back pain to flare up, they have also shown that the connection is not strong or exclusive.179 In fact, most morning stiffness and pain is not pathologically inflammatory. It’s just that IBP is the only “official” morning back pain culprit.

The pain of IBP tends to be quite severe. If it’s not actually waking you up, it’s probably not IBP, or it’s a minor case.

When should you consider the possibility of IBP? Basically if your morning back is particularly bad: nasty and very consistent morning symptoms. The diagnosis is also more likely if you have other signs of this kind of back pain. Here’s a good inflammatory back pain quiz, and here’s my own quick checklist of reasons to ask your doctor about spondyloarthritis:180

  • signs of inflammation in other body parts, especially tendons (where they attach to bone), eyes, fingers or toes, colon
  • a family history of spondyloarthritis (although it develops slowly, it’s a serious disease, so you’ll definitely know if someone in your family had it), or other autoimmmune diseases
  • you respond fairly well to NSAIDs (Aspirin, Ibuprofen, etc)
  • an infection in the weeks before your back trouble began

Technically IBP causes night back pain, not morning back pain

The morning pain of IBP is actually a case of leftover nighttime symptoms, which wear off as you wake up and get moving. When the symptoms are milder, you will mostly sleep through them, noticing them only when you wake up. But these symptoms will usually be obvious while you are still in bed, as soon as you are conscious — or even waking you up early.

In contrast, pain that you don’t notice until you actually get up and start trying to move around is less likely to be inflammatory in nature. But these are rules of thumb only, of course — there’s always lots of exceptions in biology.

PATIENT: Doctor, my back hurts when I wake up in the morning.

DOCTOR: Wake up in the afternoon then.

Doctors, do not actually trying giving this advice with patients! Joke (with visuals) via 9GAG with a hojillion likes and shares on Facebook.

Myofascial pain syndrome and/or fibromyalgia

There are two common types of widespread moderate body pain, both somewhat mysterious and controversial, both known for affecting the back more than other areas, and both known for their morning hijinks:

  1. Myofascial pain syndrome (MPS) is an infestation of too many “trigger points” — sore, aching patches of soft tissue that often seem to ease with massage and heat, but which are mostly unexplained and difficult to treat.
  2. Fibromyalgia (FM), a disease of increased sensitivity generally and decreased pain threshold, fatigue and sleep disturbance, and the “fibro fog” (mental confusion).

There’s likely lots of overlap between these conditions. Fibromyalgia isn’t really an explanatory diagnosis; it just labels a common pattern of symptoms that probably have different causes in different people. MPS is a hypothetical problem with muscle tissue that is a possible explanation for some body pain, and that type of pain is extremely common in fibromyalgia patients.

Fibromyalgia is associated with morning back pain because:

  1. People with FM rarely feel rested. The condition either causes or is caused by fragmented sleep and a lack of deep restorative sleep. Fatigue makes any pain worse, and this is probably felt mostly acutely in the morning, gradually (and imperfectly) yielding to the stimulations of the day: exercise, sunlight, coffee, people, noise and so on. The morning-ness of fibromyalgia pain could also just be “one of those things,” an unexplained rhythm of the disease.
  2. Although FM involves widespread pain by definition, back pain is a routine hot spot.

And trigger points may be associated with morning back pain because:

  1. Tissue stagnancy and postural stress seem to be a major cause of flare ups of pain of this kind, and both are an issue at night: we are often pretzeled into awkward positions in our sleep for long periods.
  2. The paraspinal muscles seem to be particularly vulnerable to trigger points. Whatever they are, they happen more in those muscles (and a few others).

The tendency of FM and MPS to affect the back in particular might be because they each affect it in their own special nasty way, or it could be two sides of the same coin. Perhaps fibromyalgia affects the back more than other areas because it causes vulnerability to trigger point … which seem to occur in the back muscles much more than other muscles.

Fibromyalgia and MPS both exist on a wide spectrum of severity. Fibromyalgia’s diagnostic criteria are notoriously tricky.182 MPS is barely even recognized as a clinical entity. Many people may have mild cases that will never be diagnosed, but morning back pain may be the tip of that iceberg — the most likely time and place for the symptoms to be felt, even if you are otherwise mostly fine.

Chronic low-grade inflammation and “inflammaging”

Everyone over 40 knows that it gets more uncomfortable to get out of bed as we age. Most people chalk it up to “arthritis,” but that’s rarely a significant factor until much later in life. Conditions like fibromyalgia and myofascial pain syndrome, as common as they are, can’t account for all of it. So what’s the problem?

A little bit of inflammation spread all over the place is a likely culprit. This might happen as a consequence of “metabolic syndrome,” a set of biological dysfunctions strongly linked to poor fitness, obesity, and aging, and possibly linked to severe chronic stress as well.183

And aging itself seems to be inflammatory (no matter how fit, skinny, and calm you are), which is known as “inflammaging.”184 So, if you’re on the far side of middle age, or you’re younger but struggling with your weight and/or major long-term stress, chronic inflammation could be your issue.

Connecting inflammaging to the curse of morning back pain

This inflammaging thing is intriguing, but does it have anything to do with morning pain or back pain specifically?

The morning link isn’t much of a reach: for whatever reason, inflammatory diseases are notoriously morning-o-centric (like the more serious inflammatory back pain mentioned above), and so it’s reasonable to assume that less serious inflammation has a thing for the morning too. We also know — only just recently — that the body can suppress inflammation at night, pumping out an anti-inflammatory protein on a schedule,185 which probably accounts for some morning pain and stiffness as the suppression wears off. No one knows anything about how to actually control that effect, but at least we know it exists.

The back link is trickier. Being overweight is less of a risk factor for back pain than most people think, so there’s no obvious association with metabolic syndrome. If there is a connection, it might be that the spinal joints are among the first structures to be affected by inflammation — and not necessarily because they are more fragile or harder working than, say, knees, but perhaps because the brain seems to be quite over-protective about the back, and more likely to raise the alarm sooner, louder, and longer.

Also, we do know that metabolic syndrome is associated with at least some common pain problems, like neck pain.186

It could also just be simple overlap: maybe these problems often just all happen at once. Inflammaging gets to everyone sooner or later, and back pain is spectacularly common, so they probably co-exist in the same people whether they have anything to do with each other or not.

Poor quality sleep and insomnia are probably major causes of morning back pain

Don’t get me wrong: some people sleep just fine and still have morning back pain. But poor quality sleep and pain tend definitely tend to go together,187 and mornings can be the roughest part of that link.

The more interesting question is the chicken/egg thing: which comes first? Pain or sleeplessness? Once you have both insomnia and pain, they surely cause each other, but one side of that equation is probably more important than the other, like a cyclist pushing much harder on one pedal than the other.

In 2017, Gerhart et al studied the which-came-first question of pain and insomnia in 105 chronic low back pain patients.188 Their subjects bravely filled out five detailed questionnaires per day for two weeks — that’s a lot of paperwork! — tracking and rating many aspects of their pain and sleep quality. The goal was to study “lagged temporal associations”: what tends to happen after what? Are bad sleeps often followed by bad days with back pain? Are rough days with back pain followed by lousy sleeps?

Yes and no to those two questions.

Poorer sleep was strongly linked to everything being worse — which is about as surprising as a dog barking at a squirrel. It’s the timing of that relationship that this study zoomed in on, and not only did a bad night clearly herald trouble across the board the next day — more pain, more disability, more doom and gloom (“catastrophizing”) — but “especially during the early part of the day.”

The relationship did not flow the other way. That is, bad nights were likely to be followed by bad days… but bad days were not followed by bad nights. The data shines a nice bright light on a simple old question. The answer isn’t much of a shock to anyone who struggles with both back pain and insomnia, but it’s important and rare to confirm this stuff with a good study.

And it gives us a compelling incentive for focussing on sleep quality as a major factor in pain generally, especially morning back pain. And almost everyone can benefit in many ways from upgrading their sleep quality. To drive that home, let’s take a closer look at the biology…

Inflammation, sleep, stress, and pain

The link between sleep and pain is based on some superficially simple inflammatory biology: bad sleep is inflammatory… and inflammation makes it harder to sleep, which is not widely appreciated. That means that sleeping badly can actually make it harder to sleep well!189 This is a vicious cycle every extremely frustrated insomniac is familiar with: being exhausted from a sleepless is not a guarantee that you will sleep well the next night.

And that vicious cycle is relevant to night and morning pain, which is known to be significantly mediated by the immune system signalling molecule interleukin-6. IL-6 and inflammation are almost synonymous — more of one means more of the other. Everyone knows that stress makes it harder to sleep, but how? It’s not just because your mind is racing — it’s because stress makes us produce IL-6, which is inflammatory, and inflammation in turn makes it harder to sleep! And then the bad sleep also makes us pump more IL-6…

And that’s why it’s important to get your sleep!

A horizontally formatted bar graph titled “When Falling Asleep Feels Easiest.” There are four graphed conditions. The first three are large bars, stretching well into the direction of “easier Zs”. They are labelled: in school, 20 minutes after lunch; at work, in a meeting; and in bed, 10 minutes before the alarm goes off. The final bar is very short and red, indicating that it is dramatically less easy to fall asleep “in bed, trying to sleep, when you need most.”

Osteomalacia (Vitamin D deficiency)

Vitamin D deficiency is probably more common than once suspected — at least 1 in 20 people in the lowest estimates,190 and possibly many more.191 Vitamin D deficiency can cause subtle widespread pain that may be misdiagnosed as fibromyalgia and/or chronic fatigue syndrome. Most notably for the purposes of this article, it can cause bone aching, particularly in the back, that is worse at night (for no clear reason). Naturally, any night pain that doesn’t actually wake you up is often noticed upon waking.

This symptom is caused by osteomalacia, which is bone weakening from malfunctioning bone building biology. The Mayo Clinic describes osteomalacia symptoms like so: “The dull, aching pain associated with osteomalacia most commonly affects the lower back, pelvis, hips, legs and ribs. The pain may be worse at night, or when you’re putting weight on affected bones.”

Other symptoms of vitamin D deficiency include: fatigue and weakness, lower pain threshold, and more acute soreness after exercise that is slower to resolve, sweating, and depression. For more information, see Vitamin D for Pain.

Awkward sleep postures

In 2015, Steffens et al found that most “back attacks” — episodes of acute back pain — occur in the first few hours of the day.192 They also identified an “awkward posture” in the two hours preceeding the attack as the second biggest risk factor (after “distracted during an activity”). Although their study wasn’t perfect, the results were too strong not to take seriously. Notably, just an “awkard posture” was quite a bit more likely to be associated with acute back pain than much more traditional bogeymen like difficult or sloppy lifting.

Based on this evidence alone, we can guess that morning back pain might be so common because sleeping is a rich source of awkward postures. In people with chronic back pain especially, it may be a routine source of minor irritation of their ongoing problems.

Awkward positions can be quite painful, even injurious. Sleeping often involves slightly awkward positions held long enough to cause sustained compression, pinching, and oxygen starvation of tissues (which may or may not have been vulnerable or irritated to begin with). The dose makes the poison: it doesn’t have to be a terrible spine position to cause trouble. Just a little awkwardness will do the job if you’re stuck that way for long enough. Although people can also carelessly tolerate postural stresses while wide awake — we generally move before they become a problem — it’s much more of a risk at night.

Sooner or later, any posture gets awkward — the trouble with tissue stagnacy regardless of position

Being still always gets uncomfortable, regardless of what position we’re in. We like to move, and sitting or lying down always gets uncomfortable in time. Forced immobilization is a potent torture method. Working in stagnant postures — like sitting in at a desk, no matter how well we do it — can start to feel more like a kind of torture as we age.193

Sleeping is an obvious potential source of tissue stagnancy, of course, but beds are more comfortable than chairs in every way, physically and psychologically. Healthy young people are relatively immune to simply being still in bed, but as we age and develop assorted vulnerabilities and sensitivities — things like fibromyalgia, myofascial pain syndrome, and chronic inflammation — it starts to become a problem.

Trigger points may be aggravated by stillness, for instance.194 Morning pain could be a wake-up call (ha ha), letting you know that you have a bumper crop of mostly asymptomatic (“latent”) trigger points that flare up overnight. In Lita’s case, trigger points could account for the consistency of her symptom timing, and for the peculiar way in which she is fine at 6am, yet can’t stay in bed past 7am, but then is fine again by 8am as she gets moving and her trigger points calm down.

Minor injury (or re-injury)

We may also cause minor injuries in our sleep, or aggravate existing minor injuries and vulnerabilities (like chronic minor back pain). Sometimes you just roll over and pinch something hard enough and quickly enough to hurt it — maybe not even enough to wake you up, but enough to feel the consequences when you do wake up. Usually this is going to be an isolated incident, but what if you keep pissing off the same vulnerable tissue?

For instance, suppose you already have minor intermittent back pain related to an old accident: it’s bothered you for years, off and on, and you’ve learned that leaning backwards is usually bad news. But then you do it in the middle of the night in your sleep, an extended lumbar spine for an hour when you weren’t even conscious — no fair! And so you wake up in moderate pain.

In other words, whatever is bothering your back in the first place can be easily and routinely aggravated — re-injured — by common sleeping positions.

I have no personal experience with back pain like this, but I know exactly what this is like from trying to sleep with a shoulder injury.195

Such incidents won’t explain all chronic, consistent morning back pain, because you’re unlikely to slightly but frequently re-injure yourself in the same way. Or maybe it’s not so unlikely: if bending your spine one way or the other is a problem, it might be easily avoided during the day, but happen to some degree most nights. Again, it’s the duration that’s the issue.

There’s much more reading you can can on PainScience.com about this topic. This book also has a chapter about “alignment” as a factor back pain, another about ergonomics, and another about optimization of sleeping posture (positioning, mattresses, pillows, and so on). And if you simply cannot get enough about posture, there also a large (free) separate article about posture in general.

Are there psychological factors that drive morning pain?

Probably not. The book Back Sense proposes that morning back pain is caused by “stressful night-time mental patterns manifesting as back pain.” I do not dismiss the role of psychology in back pain — quite the opposite! it’s a major theme in my writing about back pain.

But I think that author is indulging in a simplistic mind-body connection theory. I’ve written crankily about this kind of logic elsewhere (see Why Do We Get Sick? or Mind Over Pain). It’s a bit of a reach to say that you have such a problematic, pain-causing mental state like clockwork every morning between 6am and 7am exclusively. Applying How to Simplify Chronic Pain Puzzles to the problem, it is much (much!) more likely that there is simply a time limit on how long you can lie in bed without tissue crankiness.

What about nocturnal intervertebral disc swelling?

This is one of the classic theories about why people seem to be more vulnerable to back pain in the morning. Unfortunately, it’s too speculative to be really satisfying. But what’s the speculation?

Between every pair of vertebrae is a bit of padding, the infamous disc. They probably swell up a wee bit overnight. There are two studies that suggest this196197 (although one of them was a study of long term bed rest, and only barely applicable here). A 2016 paper speculates that disc swelling is why astronauts probably suffer from more disc herniations than they should.198 Weightlessness is undoubtedly like resting your spine, but it’s another long-term effect, so I’m not sure how relevant the astronaut angle is — it’s just a fun angle on the topic.

Discs may swell, but that doesn’t mean swelling discs hurt. Mild swelling is hardly a concern in itself, and most people don’t wake up in pain, so it’s unlikely that swelling alone is a problem. Swollen discs may hurt more in the morning if there’s already something wrong with them, such as annular tears,199 but such discs might well be uncomfortable regardless. Mildly herniated discs might swell and effectively be more herniated in the morning — but many herniations are painless, so that’s hardly a smoking gun.

It’s an interesting possibility that nocturnal disc swelling is a factor in morning back pain, but the truth is beyond our reach. For all we know, problems with discs might be much more aggravated by postural stress … or it could be a complicated combination, such as swelling that is exaggerated by postural stress but still only hurting when other problems are present.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Other possible causes of low back pain, or “No, my low back pain is really serious … ”

My big idea about the role of "simple" muscle pain in back pain coexists and competes with dozens of other theories and possible causes of persistent low back pain. Therapy is a minefield of irrational diagnoses and treatments. Patients with back pain are invariably walking around with a long list of half-baked theories in their heads about why they are in pain, worrying about other problems and complicating factors that are either mythological or wildly exaggerated: subluxations, short legs and tilted pelvises, obesity, nerve pinches, weak abdominal muscles and many others.

One of the most important things I can offer my readers is a guide to this messy list of dubious or exaggerated theories. Most chronic low back pain patients are sick and tired of the “battle of the experts,” of the many conflicting theories which cannot possibly all be correct. Nothing defines a case of “low back muscle pain” quite like the merry-go-round of theories and the treatments based on them that work only for a little while, if at all.

Almost every bad diagnosis for low back pain is a variation on the theme of fragility that isn’t so much wrong as just exaggerated. For instance, nerve impingement is a real phenomenon, but a lot less important (and scary) one than patients have been led to believe. Most people who protest that their back pain is really serious and can’t possibly be caused by “just muscle” are simply wrong. A few are correct, of course. But only a few.

The trigger points of MPS often do coexist with other problems. Indeed, they are often present because of other problems. What makes muscle such an common underestimated factor is that it complicates and mimics other problems, often even eclipsing them. It doesn’t matter what you’ve got going on, muscle is going to get involved and make it worse. So the presence of other problems, if they are there at all, means that it may be even more useful to treat your trigger points — not because they are the whole story or the "root cause" or the Big Secret to back pain, but simply because they are often the only relatively treatable part of the equation.

I’ve now worked hard to persuade you that many alleged causes for low back pain are either entirely imaginary, highly exaggerated, hard to be sure of, and/or greatly overshadowed by the more treatable and surprisingly potent discomfort of muscular trigger points.

However, I don’t want to throw the baby out with the bath water.

Disease and injury happen, sometimes they happen in the back. Hopefully they are in perspective now and it’s safer to talk about some of them. So what can happen in low backs? What goes on that’s not “just muscle”? Over the next several sections, I will address many other possible common ideas about what causes of low back pain, covering all kinds of theories: the completely bogus, the half-right, and the cromulent.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Could it be muscle strain? The muscle strain myth

When people have a sudden onset of back pain, especially during any kind of exertion, there are two classic knee-jerk interpretations:

  • Must have slipped a disc.
  • Must have strained a muscle.

But you can only have a muscle “strain” if you tried to lift something like a car and then fell to the ground howling in agony. I exaggerate, but not much. Muscles don’t tear easily.

Of course some people really do strain their back muscles. But the belief is far more common the reality. There cannot be much more than one genuine example out of a hundred claims of strain. The idea that "pulling" a muscle can explain lots of cases even when you don’t even know exactly how you supposedly did it is a misconception so common that it borders on myth.

A true strain is never subtle. It’s a trauma, and a nasty one: it involves ripped, shredded, torn muscle. That hurts a lot. It takes a lot of force to tear a muscle, and it’s a brutal experience. I’m fascinated and a little bewildered when people are married to a theory of muscle strain, but also say things like, “I’m not sure when I did it, though.” That’s like saying, “I’m not sure when I broke my leg,” or even “I’m not sure how I stubbed my toe.”

You cannot sprain ligaments or strain muscle without an obvious trigger followed by a Very Bad Day. A back muscle strain will flatten you, immediately, with absolutely no doubt about the exact moment that it occurred.

Muscle strain can occur only in the context of an intense muscular effort, and it causes severe pain immediately, almost instantly. (Mild strains may take a minute or two to become obvious.) Torn muscle will be red, hot, and swollen, and possibly bruised. Every slight contraction of the muscle will hurt, even just slight tension of the back muscles. A severe enough strain will cause some actual deformity — an obviously deformed (ripped) patch of muscle, extremely sensitive to touch in the early stages.

Muscle strain is difficult to diagnose only if it coexists with other injuries. For instance, it’s not uncommon for discs to herniate at the same time that a muscle tears, and in that awful situation you may have symptoms of two quite different tissue problems — each of them making the other less clear.

Another challenge to diagnosis is that muscle strains, even mild ones, often merge seamlessly into chronic muscle pain … long after the injury has actually healed.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

From the frying pan of injury pain to the fire of trigger point pain

If injury is the frying pan, trigger points are the fire.

Muscle pain is a routine complication of most injuries, especially in the form of trigger points. In the aftermath of an injury, the pain and stiffness of trigger points often increases rapidly, and may become so severe that the original injury becomes the least of your worries. Muscle pain is more than just a “complication” of injury — it is a common, significant long-term consequence of physical traumas.

This graph shows how trigger point pain increases & then dominates, even as injury pain is fading away.

This is all particularly true of back and neck pain, where minor and moderate injuries are common and generally feared out of proportion to their seriousness. (And it’s also a factor in serious injuries, of course.) Chronic symptoms following these injuries are often mistaken for an indicator of how serious they were to begin with.

Many factors conspire to make back and neck pain seem more serious than they are, and this is one of the most important. Although chronic muscle pain triggered by an injury is no laughing matter, there is no question that it makes injuries seem worse than they actually are. Trigger points as a complication of injury are explored in detail in the trigger points tutorial, and in this free article:

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Could it be a vicious cycle of pain-spasm-pain?

A perpetually popular idea about how pain works is that muscles go into spasm after an injury to protect the area. The alleged spasm in turn causes more pain, and then the pain causes more spasm — a vicious cycle. This is referred to as the pain-spasm-pain cycle. Unfortunately, this story is mostly an obsolete oversimplification. Although muscles can indeed complicate recovery from injury, it’s probably not due to a vicious cycle of protective muscle spasming becoming painful.

Contrary to popular professional opinion, it has been scientifically clear for decades that muscles do not “go into spasm” to protect an injured joint. Au contraire: they often go limp to protect nearby tissues.200201 The problem with pain-spasm-pain is particularly obvious in the case of a torn or “pulled” muscle — which is certainly one of the usual suspects in the back, and exactly the clinical situation where a patient is most likely to get told that. But if the muscle is actually torn, how smart would it be for the body to clench it? It would be foolish, actually — it would deepen the wound! In fact, the severe pain of a muscle strain strongly inhibits contraction of the damaged muscle.

If gross spasm was a player in low back pain, it would also presumably make it relatively easy for experienced massage therapists to feel it — but evidence discussed earlier (in "Diagnose, schmiagnose!") showed that detection accuracy is quite poor, even when just trying to identify which side is most painful.202

Odds are still good that you will hear about the pain-spasm-pain cycle if you take an injury to any kind of physical therapist today. However, the low back is the most fertile ground for the theory — the idea that the spine even needs “protecting” with spasm in the first place harmonizes nicely with our deep-seated fears of spinal fragility. And so the pain-spasm-pain is yet another in a long list of ideas about back pain that need to be debunked and discarded.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Could low back pain be an overuse injury?

Yes! But probably not. Maybe?

It’s weirdly ambiguous.

Some back pain probably does begin as an overuse or repetitive strain injury, but it’s almost a perfect mystery how common it is. It rarely seems much like it, and it may also not actually be much like it — or the seeming may just be because the back is a lot more complicated than a single fiery tendon. Even if gradual overloading is the original mechanism of injury, it may then get warped by all kind of weird back business that no Achilles tendon ever has to worry about. Once such an injury starts to hurt, it is may be hijacked by the peculiar character of back pain such that it doesn’t seem to resemble classic overuse injuries.

And some backs probably can also just become slowly but surely more irritated by an activity, and seem just as straightforward as other familiar overuse injuries do: “I did something for a long time until my back hurt,” much like playing tennis leads to tennis elbow. Consider:

  • The gardener, who stoops and kneels until her back catches fire — and then cannot comfortably garden again for weeks.
  • Or the nurse who must lift and move patients, awkwardly and often at the limits of his strength, whether rested or not, until the pain becomes overwhelming.
  • The cashier whose relentless standing slowly but surely seems to make her back hurt more and more over the years, until — boiling frog style — one day he's in enough pain that getting off his feet no longer seems to help.

Cases like these are common and seem to be a great deal like overuse injuries in overall character.

Some quick review of overuse injury basics

Overuse or repetitive strain injuries are the end result of many small injuries over time instead of a more dramatic and sudden tissue failure. Achilles tendinitis, tennis elbow, plantar fasciitis, and iliotibial band syndrome are all classic examples. More technically, these injuries all involve the relatively gradual and progressively painful failure of anatomy to adapt to load, especially tendons but also (and often overlooked) ligament, bone, and other tissues.

Exactly what fails and how it goes depends on many details, so there is a spectrum of overuse injuries ranging from quite straightforward to complex and weird. For instance, carpal tunnel syndrome is pretty clearly an overuse injury, but the only major example of one that mainly does its dirty work by affecting a nerve — and so it has many differences from a typical overuse injury.

If back pain is ever caused by overuse, it is surely at weird end extreme of that complexity spectrum — so much so that it’s often not at all obvious what's going on.

Weak points and durability: why overuse injury of the back might be rare-ish

Spines are tougher than we give them credit for — a major theme of this book. And so they may not be much of a "weak point." They are highly modular, and load is well distributed across many redundant structures, a system that is highly evolved to "take a licking and keep on ticking." It probably doesn't injures easily with repeated mild to moderate loading. It seems hard to hurt without major trauma.

All the classic overuse injuries occur in some fairly well-known vulnerable spots in human anatomy. If you’re doing something strenuous, it’s probably going to be the inflammation or irritation of one of these weak spots that stops you first. For instance, a construction worker might be (speculating) quite a lot more likely to get stopped by carpal tunnel syndrome before their back becomes a big problem.

There are no structures in the low back that seem to be obviously vulnerable to failure with common overuse. If there were, we would see a clear pattern of injury in athletes with highly repetitive and high-impact forces on the back: endurance runners would presumably be dropping like flies from low back pain, but they don’t. In those folks, it’s almost always their feet, shins, or knees.

Of all back structures, the intervertebral discs have always been considered the least adaptable and therefore the most prone to succumbing to wear and tear in jarring spots. But that’s wrong! That is not what happens. Contrary to expert assumptions and popular belief for decades, the lumbar intervertebral discs actually adapt nicely to the forces involved in running, even at volumes greater than 50km/week — “overuse” by most people’s standards. They actually get fatter and juicier! There is a sweet spot, but it’s in the pacing: almost regardless of distance, the discs adapt best to a moderate speed, a just-right amount of impact found in slow running and fast walking.203

Meanwhile, there is one population of athletes that does tend to suffer from overuse injuries of the spine, and they might be an exception that proves the rule: serious power lifters. They do have to watch out for overloading injuries, in exactly the way that you would expect from such intense compressive forces on anatomy that did not evolve for that activity. The spine is robust, sure, but good grief! I’m not saying power lifting is "dangerous," it’s not — but it’s an example of a kind of back pain that does happen, and does seem like an overuse injury… unlike most other kinds of back pain.

All of this points away from chronic overload as a common cause of back pain.

For these reasons, I spent much of my early career thinking that overload was rarely the nature of the beast in most cases where it isn't obviously the nature of the beast — the legions of people with no obvious mechanism at all for their back pain, which is by far the most common flavour of chronic back pain.

On the other hand: maybe there are a bunch of overuse injuries of the back after all?

An overuse injury doesn't have to be obvious or clear to be true, and we should probably expect it to be non-obvious and unclear in the case of . The same modularity and complexity that makes the spine quite robust may just mean that, when it fails, it does it messily compared to, say, a case of tennis elbow.

I enjoy poking holes in my own arguments. It’s kind of a hobby. Why would I say that overuse injury of the back is unlikely because the "classic" overuse injuries are simple? Ridiculous! What kind of dumdum am I? Because clearly there are some non-classic ones that are not simple! They don’t all happen to the relatively obvious, single-point-of-failure "weak spots".

For instance, bone fatigue and stress fractures are routinely overlooked in the pelvis and upper femur, despite the fact that they are rather obvious examples of overuse injuries (obvious once you know, anyway). Part of the reason that these injuries are often not diagnosed is that the pelvis is such an anatomical maze that there are lots of variations in exactly how it can fail.

Much like a spine might fail.

All tissue will fail under load: that's just biological law. It’s just a question of how much and what exactly gives up the ghost first, which is why we don’t hear about a lot of overuse injuries of the nose. But make no mistake: just because noses usually aren't subjected to repetitive strain doesn't mean it isn't possible. The question here is: are backs like noses, only theoretically vulnerable to overuse, and rarely in practice? Or are they more like pelvises: definitely vulnerable, but poorly understood?

What if the first tissue to "fail" was muscle? One of the major ideas in this book is that muscle pain can explain a lot of back pain that would otherwise be rather tricky to explain. And there is a hypothesis — just a hypothesis, but a cromulent hypothesis — is that trigger points are basically overuse injury of muscle, and intermediate and uncomfortable state between "fine" and actually tearing muscle, highly analogous to what happens to what's going on in bone before a stress fracture.

It seems highly plausible to me that in the very robust and modular spine, with no specific structure all that vulnerable to structural failure, that muscle might very frequently be the (trigger) point of failure, the weak link.

Or we could be much more conventional and make this imminently reasonable guess: overuse injuries of the spine put a strain on many anatomical structures to varying degrees, with diffuse and complex results, creating discomfort that is literally impossible to blame on a single source, because there is no single source — unlike most repetitive strain injuries, but definitely an RSI in principle.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Could it be a herniated disc?

What most people know about “discs” in their spine is that they “herniate” or slip. And it’s true — they do herniate and “slip”! Actual slippage not included — it’s just a figure of speech, and a somewhat problematic one.

And when they do herniate, they certainly can hurt, causing many symptoms.

However, disc herniations were introduced above as an over-diagnosed problem that is feared out of all proportion to its real seriousness. As you learned above, disc herniations may or may not actually be present or be the problem. They may be as trivial as a hangnail, or even painless. Even severe herniations, obvious on an MRI, may lack symptoms in some cases, and a surprising number of herniations are known to quickly resolve on their own with no treatment at all.

All of that was established above (references up the wazoo). Now that we’ve reviewed it, I still want to re-visit the topic in a different context: what if you do have a herniated disc, and it really is a problem? It’s not like they don’t exist or never hurt! They are clearly a factor in some back pain.

Diagram illustrating typical examples of disc herniations, protrusions, extrusions, and sequestrations.

Click to embiggen. All the main kinds of trouble that intervertebral discs can get into. Mostly less of a problem than people fear… but not nothing either! The images are slightly exaggerated for dramatic effect & it’s “scary” to show them… but these are things that really do happen to intervertebral discs.The good news? It looks worse than it usually is & there are many surprising reassurances.

The lumbar spine is more vulnerable to herniations for the relatively obvious reason that they are at the bottom of a stack of vertebrae. The greater the weight on them, the more likely discs are to herniate under the pressure.

If a disc bulges or bursts enough, and in the right direction, it can give a hard physical pinch to the lumbar nerve roots, causing pain and altered sensation and function throughout the pelvis and legs. And/or the disc may actually burst, spilling out a gelatinous substance like a crushed jelly doughnut — this shock-absorbing gel, when spilled into the tissues around the joint, can cause severe, deep pain. Although it’s rare, it certainly occurs, and it is one of the best examples of low back pain that is definitely not primarily caused by trigger points! There is no question that having disc jelly sprayed on your nerve roots is usually very unpleasant.

In one of the worst-case scenarios, the bulging disc pinches the entire tail end of the spinal column, causing extremely alarming symptoms such as incontinence. That is as bad as it sounds: these are medical emergencies, and no one is likely to mistake them for anything else (although, as with all things medical, the variety in the obviousness of the symptoms is startling).

The most important thing for patients to understand about disc herniations is that they clearly have the capacity to heal — and much better than most people would ever guess is possible.204 If you do clearly have a disc herniation, it should change surprisingly little about your approach to the problem.

The greater danger may not be the herniation itself, but the risk that it will soon be overshadowed by a nasty case of chronic muscle pain.

Healing of the herniation might also be retarded by a failure to address general tissue health in the region. While many herniations manage to heal with no help, it’s possible and reasonable that some cases might not heal well or quickly without appropriate treatment. In particular, supporting muscle tissue health may make a difference. There is no way to push a bulging disc back into place. But since we know they often go back into place on their own, there may well be ways to encourage them. Muscle tissue health is a potentially relevant part of the equation you can work with.

So although persistent symptoms of a disc herniation may be cause to try surgery, it’s extremely important to consider the options presented in this tutorial first, because even an old and unhealed disc herniation has the potential to finally resolve.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Facet joint syndrome and MIDs

Bracketing each pair of vertebrae are a pair of dime-sized (they are always described as “dime-sized”) joints called facet joints. These are typical slippery joints: lubricated cartilaginous surfaces, contained within a capsule. These are the joints that pop, like knuckles do, in the spine. Any of these joints can become dysfunctional, “stuck,” irritated, injured, and/or arthritically degenerated. One common pain scenario is probably a minor insult to a facet joint — sort of like a toe stub, but in your low back. When you consider how painful a toe stub can be — without doing any real damage — it’s easy to appreciate how people might over-react to the same amount of pain in the low back.

This is one example of what we call minor intervertebral derangement (MID) — a slight, uncomfortable failure of facet joint mechanics.205206 By definition, MIDs are minor, and on their own they shouldn’t cause more than a few days of pain at the worst — like recovering from a bad toe stub. However, MIDs are probably routinely complicated by fear of low back pain and the flare-up of vulnerable latent trigger points in the vicinity. In this way, many an MID may be the “cause” — the precipitating factor — of a more serious acute episode of low back pain.

Is there such a thing as a major intervertebral derangement? Almost certainly — but we would just call it an injury! The vast majority of toe stubs are harmless, but of course sometimes people actually break their toes. Similarly, if it’s common to slightly strain a facet joint, it’s also possible to more severely damage it — possibly even to the point it’s difficult to calm it down. But we would just call that a facet joint sprain. The joint surfaces might be slammed together (a “compression” sprain), and/or the connective tissue (ligaments) holding the joint together may be torn, like a tiny ankle sprain in your back. If you’ve ever had an ankle sprain, then you know how long it can take to heal.

Like muscle strains, however, genuine facet joint injury is not common, and it tends to be obvious when it occurs: a sudden, severe pain in a particular spot and associated with intense movement or impact. Hard to miss that moment! (People with facet joint injuries usually don’t have the “I can’t figure out what I did wrong” problem.) Such an injury also tends to produce pain that can be provoked — every damned time — by a specific movement. Trigger points might simulate this to some degree, but facet joint injuries are really good at it: if the joint surfaces are bruised, for instance, then they are going to hurt every single time you twist or bend the spine in such a way that they are pressed together. Every. Time. Trigger points, by comparison, rarely cause such a clear pain-movement connection.

Now that we’ve discussed most (not all) of the common, “legitimate” causes of back pain, I want to remind you again to keep them in perspective. Maybe one of these little nasties is a part of your clinical picture, and maybe it’s not. We’ve already established that it’s extremely hard to be sure. Be skeptical of any professionals who seem overconfident about your diagnosis. And bear in mind that all of these problems can interact in devious ways with trigger points, complicating diagnosis and treatment. If you have a nasty little facet joint sprain, for instance, it would hardly be surprising if, six weeks down the road, trigger points in the region started to become more of a problem than the sprain (which slowly heals). So even though you really are (or were) “injured,” remember to consider the role of muscle pain.

For the next several sections, I’ll put on my skeptic’s hat and seriously criticize some other alleged causes of low back pain that don’t make much sense or are chronically over-rated — and yet remain popular and widespread!

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Diagnostic numbing of facet joints (or the sacroiliac joint)

Photo of a syringe, isolated on white.

One possible source of back pain is the knuckle-like facet joints, as discussed above. In a sense it doesn’t even matter what, exactly, might be going wrong with a facet joint — not if you can numb the whole thing, turning off all sensation like flicking a light switch. Facet joints are specific anatomical structures with clear “edges,” much clearer than other tissues like muscle,207 so they have become the target of several minimally invasive diagnostic and treatment procedures that aim to temporarily or permanently silence any pain noise coming from them:

  • injections of various pain-relieving substances into the joint itself
  • nerve blocks which temporarily cut off sensation in the whole structure
  • destroying those same nerves

Note that everything here applies strongly to diagnosing sacroiliac joint dysfunction as well.

These procedures are all provided by medical specialists, most likely either a physiatrist or an orthopedic surgeon. Do they work? Are they safe?

Nerve blocks as a diagnostic tool

Here’s an interesting (if somewhat drastic) way to find out if a facet joint is the wellspring of spinal pain: cut off the nerve supply to the facet joint! If your pain stops, voila: presumably that’s where the pain was coming from.

This is called a “medial branch block” (MBB), or just “nerve block.” The medial branch nerves are the wee nerves that send information to the brain about tissue condition in the facet joints.208 Without those nerves, the facet joint reports nothing to the brain, and so the brain assumes all is well — a nearly perfect numbing. It’s the same as getting part of your mouth “frozen” at the dentist.

Thus, if an MBB relieves pain, “well there’s your problem.” That provides some diagnostic confidence that the facet joint is the cause of the pain.

But it’s not exactly foolproof. This practice is supported as a diagnostic tool only by scanty, conflicting scientific evidence. Some authors call it “fair” evidence, but probably shouldn’t have.209 The evidence is similar for the thoracic210 and lumbar211 spine.

Nerve blocks as a flawed diagnostic tool

Many other factors tend to confuse this method of diagnosis, which is probably why it hasn’t been validated by research. For example:

  • What if 60% of your pain is coming from one facet joint, 20% from another, and 10% from a third? And only one of them gets blocked? What if something else entirely is also causing pain?
  • What if the injection just misses? It can be difficult to be accurate with these injections. It takes skill!
  • What if the anaesthetic “leaks” into surrounding tissues and numbs the wrong thing?

It gets murkier: the amount of relief low back pain patients get from facet joint injections can be predicted by psychological factors.212 Why would that be? Numbing might provide strong enough temporary relief to create an overconfident “eureka!” And that confidence might itself deliver some pain relief, further clouding the issue, and adding up to a false positive: a misleading result that puts a spotlight on the facet joint that is actually innocent, or only part of the problem. Fortunately, the evidence suggests that this kind of confusion is probably rare, and clear relief probably means what it seems to mean.

All this complexity is also why “the effectiveness of a specific treatment cannot simply be reverse engineered to conclude what caused the pain.”213 So MBB results should be considered as a diagnostic tool for stubborn neck or back pain, but nothing is ever as simple as it seems.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Is there such a thing as a “subluxation”? Can your back be “out”?

In the winter of 2015, I went through a phase of back pain where a joint in my upper lumbar spine felt jammed: like a neck crick in my back. Every time I turned to the right, it felt like trying to close a door with something hard stuck in it. Thunk/ow. I had the vivid sensory impression that the joint was out of place.

Specifically, I have the vivid sensory impression that the joint was rotated, which is odd, because it’s anatomically impossible for lumbar joints to “rotate.” They just can’t do it. They are clearly structured to allow bending forward and backward, plus a little bit of side-bending, but intervertebral rotation just isn’t in the cards.

When a spinal joint feels out of place or jammed, there may indeed be some kind of joint dysfunction. But one fascinating, plausible possibility is that the sensation of “out-ness” may be where the explanatory buck stops: that is, maybe the joint just feels wonky. Pain distorts body image — our mental image of our own anatomy. An odd little 2008 paper demonstrated that people with back pain really feel like their vertebrae are deviated to the painful side even when they aren’t.214

It’s a straightforward idea, so simple it probably isn’t satisfying, but this phenomenon could be potent and persuasive. We are used to more or less trusting our sensations. If it feels out, we assume it must be out. But it ain’t necessarily so: sensation can be incredibly deceptive. This could be the main reason spinal joints ever feel this way. (Its illusory quality doesn’t make it any less of a problem, of course: there’s still something wrong that’s powering the illusion.)

Or maybe that joint really is out of whack in some sense. Let’s deal with that directly now: what about more literal, non-illusory subluxations?

Spinal joints can get into a few different types of trouble, but “subluxation” and spinal joints being “out” are not defined clearly enough to be useful, and are quite misleading.

“Subluxation” is mainly a chiropractic idea of some kind of spinal joint dysfunction, with many shades of meaning — too many — depending on who is talking about it. However, it is inextricably entangled with the idea of a spinal joint being “out” of place, and it is this sense of the word that needs some debunking. Many chiropractors attribute great importance to subluxation. Most believe, at the least, that subluxations cause neck and back pain, but — significantly — quite a few of them also believe that subluxations cause a wide variety of other health problems and so they “use spinal manipulation to treat visceral disease” (Homola). Subluxation theory has been both popular and controversial for many decades now, and it has never achieved medical respectability. Many experts, including quite a few chiropractors, deny that spinal subluxations exist in any meaningful sense.

It’s problematic that spinal manipulative therapy — the umbrella term for all kinds of spinal joint “adjustment” — is so often based on such a confusing and controversial concept. Subluxation has too much baggage to be a useful term. Let’s use more modern and specific terminology, and get away from the idea of spinal joints being “out.”

The controversies about subluxation theory are described thoroughly in this article:

But I’ll include one particularly important and interesting point here. As I mentioned in the introduction, chiropractors have trouble agreeing on a diagnosis of which spinal joints need “adjustment.” I do enjoy reliability studies, and this is one of my favourites. Three chiropractors were given twenty patients with chronic low back pain to assess, using a complete range of common chiropractic diagnostic techniques, the works. Incredibly, assessing only a handful of lumbar joints, the chiropractors agreed which joints needed adjustment only about a quarter of the time (just barely better than guessing). That’s an oversimplification, but true in spirit: they couldn’t agree on much, and researchers concluded that all of these chiropractic diagnostic procedures “should not be seen … to provide reliable information concerning where to direct a manipulative procedure.”215

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The role of sacroiliac joint dysfunction in back pain

The sacroiliac joint (or SI joint or SIJ) is the large, sturdy joint on either side of the triangular sacrum. The sacrum sits in the pelvic like a keystone at the top of a stone arch — it’s even the same shape. The SIJs connect the pelvic bones (the ilium specifically) to the sacrum, and support the weight of the whole upper body.

The SIJ is more like a seam than a joint. It is a synovial joint, which means it’s in a capsule and has slimy cartilaginous surfaces just like a hip or knee joint, but almost everything else about it is strange: two large, irregular surfaces that remind me of the two halves of an oyster coming together.

It also only moves about as much as an oyster . It does move, but only a couple millimetres of sliding and a couple degree of twisting.216 On the one hand, any functional movement has the potential to be uncomfortably messed up. On the other hand, people with fused SIJs suffer no obvious impairment (but might still be somewhat more vulnerable to arthritis in the adjacent joints).

Natural sacral joint fusions

Sometimes the sacrum is fused to the lowest lumbar vertebra: a lumbosacral transition vertebra. “LSTV is the most common congenital anomaly of the lumbosacral spine.” In about a thousand patients studied by Sekharappa et al, it was about twice as common in patients who had sought spinal surgery as it was in patients with no spinal complaint (about 14-16% of patients, instead of 8%).217 The study also identified a “definite causal relationship” with degeneration of the disc above the LSTV.

Fusion of the sacroiliac joint is closely related and even more common, as high as half the population by the age of 80, and freakishly more common in men than women for some reason, in 287 subjects studied by Dar et al.218

These joints — the lumbosacral, and the sacroiliac — are so sturdy and immobile that, in some people, they cross the line and develop into a solid block. Talk about a stiff back! Literally!

Angela’s story: “Like fine gravel or sand in my pelvis”

Angela Zaleski woke up one morning in her mid-forties, took a step, staggered, and sagged to the ground — surprised more than hurt, but unable to support her weight on her right leg. The trouble was in a very specific location, straight south of her low right back dimple, on the right edge of her tailbone. “It was like having a rock in my shoe — but instead of one rock in my shoe, but it was like fine gravel or sand in my pelvis. I couldn’t settle onto it. It didn’t hurt, but I could tell it was going to hurt if I wasn’t careful.”

It was out of the blue. She hadn’t hurt herself that she knew of, had never felt anything like this before, and didn’t have any experience with back pain.

She gingerly stood up on her left leg and started “basically just squirming and testing weight-bearing on my right”… and it felt like it calmed down and settled and started to allow weight within a couple minutes. It was like the joint had been temporarily screwed up. She thought that was probably the end of it, just one of those weird body things.

But it was back the next time she stood up from sitting for a while.

And it was back the next morning. And then all the mornings ever since.

She quickly learned that she could never be still for long without the problem “recharging.” But she also learned that it usually wasn’t a big deal — no one incident was hard to cope with. A little squirming and shifting around, and the joint would settle down and started working. She got used to the “crunchiness” of the joint. “I can pretty much just walk it off now. It’s still there, and the first few steps are a bit wobbly and careful, but it’s not that bad.”

Unfortunately, the discomfort spread. As the weeks dragged on and it didn’t go away, she began to ache all around the problem, but especially just above in the pit of the low back. That pain eventually ramped up to the point where it was more distressing than the grinding in her SIJ, which continued to be a constant minor annoyance. As of 2019, she’s been managing this for about two years, with no improvement. She seems to be able to substantially beat back her symptoms — the aching — with trigger point therapy, but the aching always come back … probably because the SIJ grinding never leaves.

It’s hard to account for an experience like this without concluding that her sacroiliac joint “might be screwed up somehow,” and probably in a hypermobile kind of way, but exactly how and why remains a mystery. Although I’ve encountered many similar stories over the years, hers is the direct inspiration for adding this chapter to the back pain book.

Is SIJ “dysfunction” a real thing?

Caution, reservation, and even skepticism should be exercised on the part of the surgeon attempting to diagnose (let alone treat) this condition. It is very difficult to invalidate a treatment for diseases that cannot be reliably diagnosed. Not surprisingly then, therapeutic options are varied, poorly objectified, and remain within the same twilight zone as the treatment of lumbar facet pain.

Lonser et al, 2017, Neurosurg Clin N Am219

I believe it is real, but there are also a lot of caveats.

An honest translation of the word “dysfunction” is usually just “screwed up somehow.” Broadly speaking, loose, stuck, and/or inflamed sacroiliac joints surely do happen, but there are a variety of ways to get into those states and most of them are poorly understood. For instance, knowing that it’s “hypermobile” isn’t half the battle. It might be hypermobile because of several quite different clinical situations:

  • an injury
  • a hypermobility disorder (a genetic condition, and quite common)
  • pregnancy (which loosens ligaments)
  • a postural habit that’s been yanking on some ligaments for thirty years

Many experts do indeed doubt the validity and reliability of a diagnosis of SIJ dysfunction. Most skepticism is focused on the dubious idea of SIJ subluxation, an SIJ that is “out,” which is even more unlikely in the SIJ than in the rest of the spine, because the joint is so massive and sturdy. Even when hypermobile, we’re still only talking about quite small movements. The joint might move too much, but it’s not likely to spend any significant time “out” of position. It’s worth quoting this again:

I talked to a trauma surgeon that had been to a workshop where they talked about the sacrum being “out of place.” He just said this is ridiculous: we see people who are in motor vehicle accidents with every bit of their body smashed but the sacroiliac joint is intact. It is so strong.

Peter O’Sullivan, Professor of Musculoskeletal Physiotherapy at Curtin University, Perth, Australia

Skeptics are mostly concerned that the importance of SIJ dysfunction is exaggerated, not that it doesn’t exist at all, for several reasons:

  • As with so much chronic pain, it may be just one factor in a stew of complex drivers of pain and pain chronicity, and subtle biological vulnerabilities may be the more fundamental issue.
  • There are other likely causes of pain in the same area, of course.
  • How reliably or meaningfully can it be diagnosed? For instance, while surgical fusion of the SIJ might be helpful for some back pain, you have to be able to identify the patients who need it — which seems to be a major stumbling block.220
  • Even if you could diagnose it reliably, can you actually do anything about it? As with so many structural problems, even if it was a good idea to tinker, it might just not be possible. It pretty clearly cannot be “adjusted” manually.221 Even if you bolt it together with titanium screws, you can’t necessarily going to stop it from shifting around,222 any more than you can hold a squirmy puppy completely still. Life doesn’t like to be immobilized!

As we saw with Angela Zaleski’s case, however, sacroiliac joints do get into trouble and can become the well-spring of back pain.

Diagnosing sacroiliac joint pain

It is probably not possible to reliably detect abnormal sacroiliac joint movement by feeling or watching the joint move,223 or only in the most extreme cases. Remember, the joint can rotate only a couple degrees and slide only a couple millimetres.224 If you are diagnosed based mainly on a simple physical exam, be skeptical!

Despite all the uncertainty, it’s relatively straightforward to diagnose sacroiliac joint pain based on the clinical picture. First of all, the location is pretty consistent and revealing: if you have pain along the edge of the sacrum, there’s a pretty good chance that it’s sacroiliac joint pain. It’s most likely to be irritated by sitting and forward bending. And it is more likely to be SIJ pain if there aren’t any other major kinds of symptoms (e.g. numbness, tingling, shooting pain down the leg).

There are certainly several other possibilities to be aware of, and the big one is probably just muscle soreness in the immediately adjacent gluteals.225

Imaging can also fairly easily reveal SI joints that are in the greatest amount of trouble from chronic inflammation.

And the sacroiliac joint is a big target: it’s fairly easy to inject it with a pain killer, and see if that relieves a bunch of pain. Although I wouldn’t start with that, it’s definitely worth considering. See “Diagnostic numbing of facet joints” for more information about this approach.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Are you crooked? The alignment and posture villains: short legs, pelvic tilts, and spinal curves

Nearly all backs could make an instant claim on the warranty, if there were one. If [God] were responsible for back design, you’ll have to concede that it wasn’t one of His best moments and must have been a deadline rush job at the end of the Six Days.

Unintelligent Design: Why God Isn’t as Smart as She Thinks She Is, Robyn Williams, 2007. p. 63

Poor posture and misalignment — especially short legs and pelvic tilts — are widely believed to cause back pain.

How does that work exactly?

There is really only one simplistic idea here: posture causes arthritis. Crookedness applies small stresses to the spine over long periods, eventually causing painful degenerative changes. Supposedly.

Make no mistake: when people talk about posture causing back pain, “arthritis” is what it boils down to. They are guessing that you are slowly ruining your spine by being a bit off-kilter, and it’s a garbage guess. Even if you actually are off-kilter, it’s probably mostly harmless, and definitely does not account for serious back pain.

We’ve already thoroughly established that arthritic changes have very little to do with back pain, so it wouldn’t matter even if posture did causes trivial arthritic changes.

But it doesn’t even do that! Crookedness probably isn’t the explanation for any back pain at all, let alone a lot of it.

Short legs: the all-time classic example of a crooked theory

The short leg thing is by far the best-known example of a cause of postural stress — and I think it’s also among the most wrong and common modern back pain pseudo-diagnosis, constantly foisted on patients by healthcare professionals and other patients. It sells a lot of orthotics and heel lifts. I have heard countless statements like “my last therapist told me I have a short leg,” and I am certainly not the only one. Dr. James Noake:

“Just had a patient who’s had 19 sessions of chiropractic treatment, and been told he has a 14mm leg length discrepancy. He’s not better obviously. Can we stop with this now? I’m tired of unravelling this BS in clinic, especially on Friday afternoon.”

I have been asking myself “Can we stop with this now?” since the early 2000s, so I guess the answer is “nope.” This leg-length of nonsense — from chiropractic or any other profession — isn’t going anywhere soon. No matter how many smart people gripe about it.

Why is leg length unimportant?

Usually the short leg is either trivial, less than a centimetre, or entirely imaginary. It was a routine professional experience for me in my massage years to be unable to detect a leg length difference diagnosed by other therapists. I do not think it was because I was any less talented or observant therapist. I think the diagnosis had more to do with therapists wanting to seem biomechanically savvy than detecting any actual differences in leg length.

It is not hard to detect leg length differences. This is not, as they say, rocket science. The anatomical landmarks are clear. Either the leg is longer or it is not. If the difference isn’t clear, it can’t possibly be significant.

(Or … maybe it is difficult? In 2016, a couple of veteran chiropractors were unable to produce the same results using two common methods of assessing leg length.226 Ruh roh!)

It’s all a moot point for low back pain, because a 1984 study — old but good, published in English medical journal Lancet — clearly establised that leg length differences do not cause back pain.227 While it is obvious that a significant difference in leg length might still be a factor in back pain, significant leg length differences are rare, and would still be only a factor in low back pain — a factor that would probably express itself primarily in the form of muscle pain.

What about amputees? Extremes are interesting and instructive, and back pain is indeed common in people who have lost a leg, and it’s common to try to treat it by adjusting their prostheses. These are people who basically have adjustable legs! But a 2015 study found that there was in fact no length difference to correct in amputees with back pain.228

Not that you need an artificial leg to adjust your leg length. If short legs caused back pain, surely it would be a slam dunk to treat the problem with orthotics or heel lifts? But orthotics have no obvious benefit for back pain patients.229 The only studies with positive results have serious flaws.230 (Orthotics don’t even prevent the lower limb injuries they are most often prescribed for.)231

The friends of leg-length differences

The leg-length theory of back pain is the best-known example of a whole bunch of similar and related diagnoses.

A close runner-up in popularity is the pelvis-outta-whack theory — tilted or twisted — which goes nicely with the leg length difference theory: each can cause the other, allegedly. Pelvic misalignment isn’t completely mythological, but its importance in back pain is. Several flavours of the crooked pelvis diagnosis are common — and equally suspect. It comes down to this: a pelvis tilt too small to reliably detect in a mirror is very unlikely to be a primary cause of low back pain. Backs are just not that fragile.

And then there’s lordosis.

Lordosis

The lordotic curve is the curve of the low back. There’s a wide range of what’s normal, and it’s also constantly changing — in most people, with or without pain, regardless of their age, sex, height, or weight. Measure it objectively with a curve-o-meter five times in a row, and you are likely to give five different results, as shown in a nifty little 2018 experiment.232

Lordosis” refers to a prominent lordotic curve, but it’s unclear how much curve is too much curve. It’s a variation on the pelvis-outta-whack theory and nearly as popular a postural villain as a short leg. The Internet is awash with back pain cures based on this simplistic idea that flattening the back will help (with the alleged bonus of flattening bellies at the same time, which often gets advertised as perk). Rolfer Todd Hargrove shares my feelings about this idea and how pervasive it is:

The cure for pain is usually presented as involving one or more of the following elements: stretching the hip flexors, strengthening the glutes and abs, or making a conscious effort to suck in the gut or otherwise modify the pelvic angle in standing. There are many different variations on this prescription of course, but you can find some version of it almost anywhere you look in the world of manual therapy and corrective exercise.

A kernel of truth?

Bad ideas that have no truth in them at all usually don’t last. It’s the ones that have a kernel of truth in them that really have legs.

There’s almost certainly a kernel of truth in the concern about lordotic curve. Being bipedal probably is a little more challenging to spines than being quadrupedal. Old data from studying “primitive” people in societies where people squat a great deal — eliminating the lumbar curve for large percentages of their lives — showed fewer signs of strain on their lumbar vertebrae than our more “civilized” postures produce.233 The authors also note that degeneration is more prevalent in animal spines in the places you’d expect: right where the curves are sharpest, and where the gravitational strains are the greatest.

But even if people who stand up more really do have more spinal degeneration, by no means does this mean that they have more pain — there are other major factors in the mix. Nor does it mean that you could do anything about it by trying to adjust your posture now, short of switching to a squatting-based lifestyle for a decade — and even that might not work, for a dozen reasons.

There is enough evidence about the curve concern to put it out of our minds. Either there is no connection between lordosis and back pain at all, or it’s not enough of a connection to worry much about.234235236237 Whatever weak correlation might exist could easily just be a postural response to pain, rather than the other way around.238

And remember, Schmidt showed pretty clearly that you can’t measure it reliably, not even with an objective gadget — let alone the biased eyeballs of a massage therapist keen to find something to “work” on. And you cannot diagnose or fix what cannot be measured.

All that data torpedoes a number of back pain cure empires.

Anatomical illustration of a side view of the spine with the lumbar curvature highlight and labelled “lordosis.”



Spines are dynamic

Ask the same person to stand for a postural assessment several times in a row & that curve will spontaneously change to something measurably different at least a couple times, without rhyme or reason — regardless of pain, age, height, weight, or sex.

What about hamstring tightness? To be continued

The hamstrings are the most popular main scapegoat for a wonky pelvic tilt, and it motivates a great deal of hamstring stretching. I will cover that topic in full later in the guide, framed in the context of stretching as a treatment.

Why don’t pregnant women tip over?

Photo of a pregnant woman, standing. The photo is rotated so that the woman is tilted significantly forward.

I will now exploit pregnant women to make the point of this chapter in a more interesting way. In the quest to make this subject matter as amusing as it is informative, I ask you: Why don’t pregnant women tip over?

It is a nifty anatomical fact that women have larger, stronger posterior lumbar joints,239 which is almost certainly a biomechanical feature that evolved to cope with major combined stresses of a large, awkwardly off-centre weight and leaning backwards to keep from falling over.240

What are the odds that this evolutionary adaptation makes women immune to the back strain caused by pregnancy? Well … nil! Even today, even with tougher spines, pregnant women suffer high rates of low back pain. Ask any pregnant woman how her back feels.

What we take from this is that the importance of spinal curvature is moderated by evolution. We can clearly see that deviations from average spinal curvature are a factor in back pain, or women probably would never have evolved an adaptation to cope with it. On the other hand, the same adaptation shows that both men and women are probably adapted enough that spinal curvature alone cannot be a deal breaker — if it were, we would have evolved to cope with it.

Another way of putting it: evolution doesn’t care if you have back pain, just as long as you can breed … but it always makes sure that you can do that much. It is easy for nature to saddle us with biomechanical features that are uncomfortable and imperfect, but still keeping us mostly protected from biomechanical features that are routinely crippling. Here’s an interesting passage about this:

We often say the reason people get lower back pain is because we became bipeds and being a biped is a stupid way to use your back. But actually that doesn’t make any sense, because if back pain is so difficult, such a challenge, natural selection surely would have acted to lessen the prevalence and severity of back pain. [Indeed, it probably has: the Whitcome paper on women’s lumbar spines — with Lieberman’s name on it — shows pretty clearly that we do have such adaptations. — Paul] In fact, if you start asking people who work with hunter-gatherers, most people say yes, actually come to think of it, I don’t really recall anybody saying that they had back pain. I’ve never seen anybody have back pain in the hunter-gatherer context.

Dr. Dan Lieberman, evolutionary biologist, Brains Plus Brawn

This section just gives you a glimpse into a much bigger rant about “structuralism,” which is what I call the excessive emphasis on alignment, posture, and biomechanical factors in physical therapy. For much more information, see Your Back Is Not Out of Alignment. But it’s all summed up by this conclusion from a large scientific study that tried and failed to find connections between structural problems and back pain:

… structural factors such as the size of the lumbar lordosis, pelvic tilt, leg length discrepancy, and the length of abdominal, hamstring, and iliopsoas muscles are not associated with the occurrence of low back pain.241

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Do you really need to lose some weight?

One of the simplest of all idea about low back pain is that extra weight is a factor and losing it will help. Do you need to lose some weight? Does being heavier hurt backs?

It’s more likely that it hurts to be un-fit. Any trouble with weight is probably more metabolic than mechanical. (I’ll circle back to this.) But first, while the pregnancy stuff from the last section is fresh in your mind, because there’s an obvious connection …

Pregnancy is a kind of “temporary obesity” — sorry, ladies, an unkind but factual phrasing — so what applies to pregnancy probably roughly applies to obesity. And men are likely a little more vulnerable in the lumbar spine to weight gain, because they lack the impressive and handy spinal adaptations of women — if it helps women a little, it must harm men a little.

However, the differences are minor statistical differences in rates of back pain — perhaps so minor that they are difficult to even detect. There is no obvious epidemic of pregnant women and overweight people who are crippled by low back pain while their skinny friends and neighbours are blissfully back painless. Lots of skinny people are afflicted by back pain.

Leaving the useful example of pregnancy behind, what does the science say more directly about weight and back pain? Nothing conclusive — which is what you’d expect if it’s not a big deal. An obvious connection would be … obvious. A minor connection? Obviously harder, but still: you’d think researchers could work this one out. How hard can it be?

Amazingly hard, of course. It’s science. Science is hard!

Research on the link between back pain and obesity

Hussain et al found a clear link, but not for the reason everyone assumes.242 Dario et al found no link at all, despite looking extremely carefully.243

Hussain et al. shows evidence of the metabolic roots of back pain, and maybe other kinds of chronic pain as well. The majority (82%) of 5000 Australians reported back pain on a questionnaire, and in 27% of them it was bad enough to be disabling. When compared to their fat mass fat distribution — known indicators of metabolic disorders — a clear pattern emerged: back pain intensity and disability go up with measures of fat mass and distribution.

This data does not suggest that weight is a “mechanical” problem — greater weight causing greater stress on spinal joints — but rather that “systemic metabolic factors associated with adiposity play a major role in the pathogenesis of LBP.”

This is a great example of what I mean when I say (and I often do) that we need to look beyond biomechanics — way beyond — to the messy “wet” factors in chronic pain.

That sounds very much like the last word, but this is musculoskeletal medicine, where nothing is straightforward.

No link at all in a big twin study

Specifically because “current evidence remains unclear,” Dario et al. tried to get everything right: a long-term study of over a thousand initially healthy people, measuring both the overall amount of fat as well its distribution, and — this is important — using twins to control for “the possible effects of genetic and early shared environmental factors.” This is an advanced approach.

And the results? They found … nothing: “No increase in the risk of chronic LBP was found for any of the obesity-related measures.” They checked six ways from Sunday, and “obesity-related measures did not increase the risk of developing chronic low back pain.”

So where does that leave us? Still in the woods! The relationship between back pain and obesity has always been — and remains — a bit of a question mark. My best guess is that there is no link, or only a weak one, and it’s explained as much or more by metabolism than the stress applied to your spine by extra weight. This fits right in with many other counter-intuitive truths about back pain.

Despite a seemingly obvious connection between back pain and obesity, there’s nothing obvious about it.

A large example from life

I had a middle-aged client who was particularly large, about 200 kilograms — truly obese. She normally saw me for other issues, but one day came to my office with severe low back pain, notably the first she had ever suffered. Fortunately, she is a wonderfully self-aware person, and trusting — she didn’t leap to the conclusion that it had to be because she’s too heavy, and she immediately accepted the idea that her back pain was probably caused mostly by irritated muscle. I was grateful that she had come to me first, because a physician would almost certainly have alarmed her with grave warnings about her weight causing spinal degeneration or vulnerability to injury. While that is probably a reasonable concern over the long term, I did not believe that it was the most important or immediate cause of her pain. I treated her for muscle pain, and she was out of pain in one week. She remained pain free for at least three years before I lost track of her.

A point of anatomy

The importance of the spine as a weight-bearing structure is exaggerated. Although it does indeed bear weight, it’s not really a matter of “stacking” vertebrae. The strain of supporting extra weight is, in fact, significantly muscular — muscular action, via a concept called “tensegrity,” is actually a much more important principle of spinal support. In effect, the lumbar spine is more like a spring than a stack of bricks. For more information, see the (surprisingly fun) article Ten Trillion Cells Walked Into a Bar.

So is weight a factor? Almost certainly. Is it a critical factor? No. Weight differences within a reasonable range are almost certainly not a big deal.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Is back pain a disease of civilization? A modern lifestyle disease?

Is back pain actually a modern problem, a disease of civilization and modern lifestyles, like diabetes and heart attacks? Or is that just a cynical myth about the perils of modernity?

I started asking that in about 2005, a few years into my career as a massage therapist (and in the earliest stages of writing the first edition of this book). I had been busily telling my patients what every self-respecting alternative medicine practitioner was supposed to: sitting is evil, postural sloppiness is ruining us, your core is weak, and “primitive” people don’t have these problems.

I mean, duh, we evolved without chairs! Paleo-posture is obviously superior!

As a somewhat low-“ranking” healthcare provider, this seemed like a valuable pearl of slightly subversive wisdom that I could offer to my clients, something those “mainstream” doctors wouldn’t bestow upon them. Try to act more like a hunter-gatherer!

So practical! 🙄

As a side effect of my skepticism on other fronts, it occurred to me that I just had no idea if the claim was true. Do “primitive” people actually get less back pain? How did I think I knew that anyway? Well, because I had heard it, obviously.244

But I needed data! I will now take you on a five-minute tour of the fifteen-year journey I took to get a clear answer to this question.245 Happily, there is one.

Is back pain on the rise in recent history?

Many researchers do seem to believe that low back pain is a modern problem. For instance, Waddell writes, “Observations of natural history and epidemiology suggest that low-back pain should be a benign, self-limiting condition, that low back-disability as opposed to pain is a relatively recent Western epidemic … .”

In 2008, Martin et al found that, “The estimated proportion of persons with back or neck problems who self-reported physical functioning limitations increased from 20.7%… to 24.7% … 1997 to 2005,”246 which shows some kind of growing problem, and that in turn might mean that back pain is worse now than it was in the more distant past.

A Spanish 2001 study showed that “serious” musculoskeletal complaints (including a great deal of back pain, presumably) had increased significantly from 1993.247

Harkness et al did a nice job in 2005 of comparing rates of musculoskeletal pain (including low back pain) 40 years apart in the northwest of England, and found a large increase.248 And they also pointed out that the appearance of a recent increase “could be partly explained by the ‘worried well’. The ‘worried well’ are those patients who are concerned about their health, and attend their GP to seek reassurance about their well-being.” A great example of how hard statistics can “lie.”

You can’t confident extrapolate from those points to conclude that back pain is a modern problem, though.

I could probably update these citations with some more modern ones, but it’s too indirect anyway. This was just the only kind of data I could find at the time.

Evidence from related concepts

When I went looking for more direct and relevant evidence, I simply couldn’t find it. Maybe I didn’t have the research skills. Maybe the evidence didn’t exist! That seemed plausible. There’s not enough evidence about anything in musculoskeletal medicine, and virtually none about unusual populations.

Body pain + basic cultures = thin results in a PubMed search!

I did find clear evidence that excessive sitting is not a major risk factor for back pain, and I publicly reversed my position on that.

I also became a skeptic about the clinical significance of posture more generally.

And I became convinced that “core weakness” isn’t really a thing, and doesn’t doom us to back pain in any case. (The next chapter is all about that.)

All that strongly suggested that back pain probably isn’t a disease of modern lifestyles, but it was still all a bit tangential, "circumstantial" evidence.

And that was about as good a job as I could do for a long time. I still did not know, specifically, about the prevalence of back pain in cultures that didn’t spend their days sitting at computer stations. (And I had much else to think about.)

I needed even better, more directly relevant data.

Even better, more directly relevant data

Someone else did the work for me! Now I can just cite his work, like this: in 2017, James Steele, PhD, wrote a paper about how back pain is probably a human problem and not a modern human problem.249 He thoroughly reported on the evidence that I’d failed to find. Yahtzee! Thank you, James!

Surprise surprise, the data says that back pain is not a disease of civilization. Dr. Steele: “LBP is common to almost all populations examined and that outliers are easily explained by their reluctance to report pain and instead to rely on traditional healing methods.”250 The data isn’t perfect or complete, but it’s good enough for high confidence. There’s either no difference at all, or not a major one.

And it gets better. It’s not just most kinds of humans alive today that get back pain, it has probably been most kinds of humans ever:

We have focused on extant populations thus far, both ‘modern’ and ‘traditional.’ However, there is evidence of lumbar spine damage and degeneration potentially pursuant to LBP in many examples of extinct species of Homo and other early human populations. These include Homo erectu, Homoheidelbergensis, ancient Egyptians and Nubians, ancient Chinese populations and Ötzi the Iceman. This included disc herniation, spondylolisthesis, scoliosis, osteophytosis, rheumatoid arthritis, osteoarthritis, lumbar spur formation, and ankylosing spondylitis all of which have the potential to cause LBP.

… it would seem that the cultural environment, despite varying considerably between populations, has little impact upon LBP prevalence.

Intriguingly, for Dr. Steele, this is all just a premise for an interesting hypothesis about how humans are just biologically vulnerable to back pain.251 But it’s a major premise and supported well. Indeed, looking at the evidence he assembled, I am humbled by my failure, circa 2010, to find, um, any of it.

So that’s that. Steele 2017 will be my go to reference for this point for a long time to come. Back pain isn’t a modern disease? Steele 2017. Primitive people get just as much back pain as programmers? Steele 2017. Back pain is huge annoyance for all kinds of humans? Steele 2017!

Possible objections to this conclusion

How could I be wrong about this? Because, of course, I could be wrong about this!

The most likely objection that I am aware of is that modern hunter-gatherer populations are actually quite different from our actual prehistoric ancestors. I have no doubt that is a big deal for certain kinds of research questions. I am not so sure it’s a big deal for this one.

People trapped in chairs all day (hi there!) fantasize about how much more robust their backs might be if only we were much more active, if only we had a much greater diversity of physical challenges every day. And that absolutely exists in other modern populations! That comparison is possible, and that’s what the conclusion is based on.252

It also just seems somewhat unlikely to me that prehistoric populations would be protected from back pain by some X-factor that modern highly-active populations do not enjoy. But it’s untestable in any case.

If you are aware of any other objection to the claim that back pain is not a modern disease, please let me know.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Is it core weakness?

Before reading about the science, please get into the mood by watching this fine 29-second Japanese advertisement for a core strength training machine, the Wonder Core. It just keeps getting better as you watch! (Oh, and: earworm warning — that little jingle is infectious!)

Laughter is the best core workout. (If the video has been removed, which has happened several times over the last few years, just search YouTube for “wonder core ad” and you’ll find it easily.)

It seems everyone and their dog thinks that the root of all low back pain is a consequence of weak abdominals and other core musculature around the pelvis, which in turn strongly implies a deep-seated belief that spines are “unstable” if you don’t work hard to shore them up. But instability is just the presumed consequence of many of the biomechanical bogeyman we’ve already shown to be not so scary, if not entirely imaginary. And back pain isn’t caused by instability any more than by the things that supposedly make us unstable. Dr. O’Sullivan in 2021, summarizing his 2012 editorial in the British Journal of Sports Medicine:253

“I used to believe instability in the lumbar spine was a thing — even published about it — but the evidence just didn’t stack up.”

A nugget from the editorial:

“It is now clear that there is little evidence to support the view that ‘instability’ underpins the basis of disabling non-specific chronic low back pain. There are no studies that demonstrate a clear relationship between spinal or pelvic mobility, degenerative processes, pain and disability.”

Core strengthening is a fad that started in the mid-90s based on a reasonable theory, but it “came to dominate thinking as an accepted truth way before we had a good answer about whether it worked” (Neil O’Connell, a snippet of a terrific longer passage254). The word “core” seems to suggest something fundamental, foundational — it has a confidence-inspiring connotation.

The belief in the power of core power is now so widespread that it dominates all thinking about exercise therapy for low back pain: there are really no other serious contenders. Nearly every popular kind of exercise therapy for low back pain is some variation on the idea that your core needs training. It is the conceptual justification for basically all exercise prescriptions for the back, whether it is spelled out or not.

Therapeutic exercise for the back is core strengthening. They are effectively synonymous.

Core strength has a major logical problem

A lot could be said about this annoyingly persistent theory of low back pain, but I can dismiss the subject with one dollop of logic: I have seen lots of low back pain patients who clearly have perfectly good core strength, people who can do a lot more crunches than I can, and I have also seen lots of patients who’ve never had low back pain and yet they have never done a crunch in their lives. Thus, as with weight, “core weakness” in itself is probably a minor factor in low back pain, but it cannot possibly be a critical factor.

But that’s just my clinical experience. What actually matters is that it is validated by the science to date. In late 2008, Physical Therapy published a review of all research on “motor control exercises,”255 which are one particularly “fancy” way of working on core stability, physiotherapy exercises that try to teach you to improve the coordination of your core and low back musculature. It’s like trying to learn how to wiggle your ears — difficult, but fiddly! These exercises are a popular variation on core stability training. However, all the research that had been done up to then was hopelessly unexciting. When scientists have studied a therapy more than a dozen times, you hope to see nice clear evidence that it really does the trick! No such luck. The authors conclude that “motor control exercise is superior to minimal intervention,” a somewhat disingenuous phrase to lead with. What they are really saying is, “Motor control exercise is better than nothing.” Which it is. But guess what? It is also “not more effective than manual therapy or other forms of exercise”!

So, in other words, you could have done any kind of exercise and gotten roughly the same benefit. Whoop-de-doo!

What that tells me is that motor control exercises are not exactly laser-focused on fixing any root cause of low back pain. If low back pain was truly caused by poorly coordinated action of the core musculature, you would expect to see motor control exercises really make a difference — way more of a difference than “other forms of exercise.” But they don’t!

Undoubtedly people with low back pain do indeed have impaired coordination256257 — and an impaired sense of touch,258 interestingly — but that’s hardly surprising. Do you feel like you have good precise motor control of the centre of your body when your back is screaming? I know I don’t! Despite all the fuss, researchers have never been able to prove that poor coordination is actually the cause of low back pain — and I don’t think any sensible person is surprised by that, because obviously, it could easily be exactly the other way around.

This is just a taste of this subject. Because the perceived importance of core strengthening is so high, it will be discussed in more detail below in the context of treatments and the question, “Should you bother?” I’ve written about it briefly here just to introduce you to the disappointing reality, and to emphasize sooner rather than later in the tutorial that this chestnut of low back pain “wisdom” is blown way out of proportion. Neil O’Connell (again) put it like this in a short 2011 article:

The problem is that, whichever way you slice the data, the effects of exercise are small to moderate at best (kind of the therapeutic equivalent of beige slacks: better than nothing but underwhelming).

Core neck strengthening?

Core strengthening for the neck is called deep cervical flexor training, and it’s a scientifically bankrupt analogy to core strengthening for the back. It’s “based” on some weak evidence that people with neck pain have weak deep cervical flexors, which is an unproven premise that the weakness is a cause of neck pain rather than just a symptom, and a smattering of literally corrupt (actually fraudulent!) evidence that strengthening the DCFs helps. The state of DCF training is about as depressing an example of the shabby state of rehab science as you can cough up. Full details and references in my neck pain book (of course). For our purposes here, it’s enough to just say that it’s a train wreck, and suggests that “core” strength isn’t an important consideration in any part of the spine.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Is it all in your feet? Foot-o-centric low back pain theories

High heels, flat feet, and bad shoes are all semi-regular but minor blame magnets for low back pain. You can safely ignore them all with the same logic that we’ve used on several other theories discussed so far. If we are generous, they might be a minor factor, yes. But the thing? The actual main problem? You know the likely answer by now, but here’s an interesting recent example from high-heeled science…

A meticulous 2010 study showed that the body adapts effectively and minimally to high heels, producing similar functional results.259 The most interesting implication of their results is simply that “muscle structure may adapt to a chronic change in functional demand” — which might seem obvious, but that little bit of science has been hard to nail down over the years, and this is a good piece of the puzzle.

Chronic high-heel wearers do have shortened calf muscles, stiffer Achilles tendons, and a smaller ankle range of motion, but these changes “seem to counteract each other since no significant differences in static or dynamic torques were observed.” In other words, high heel wearers are not progressively disabled: their ankles work fine, just differently. This doesn’t mean there’s no conceivable harm (for instance, Kerrigan found evidence of harm to the knees), but it does tend to downgrade concern on the topic.

Most women who wear heels probably will not notice any obvious connection with back pain. If you do, I would suggest taking a break from them — but I would also recommend that ultimately the problem is not that “heels are dangerous to the low back,” but that “the back is too vulnerable to a minor stress.”

For a specific and entertainingly bizarre example of a questionable foot-o-centric theory of body pain, see the appendix to The Not-So-Humble Healer, “I have been humble for 2 decades now.”

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Your problem is that you’re having “too much fun”

This sub-topic is mostly unique to low back pain. There’s just something about back pain that makes doctors particularly likely to tell patients that they may have to stop doing the things they love to do. It comes up with some overuse injuries too, but back pain really inspires this attitude, like the classic medical joke…

“Doctor, my shoulder hurts when I lift my arm like this.”

“Well, then, don’t lift your arm like this.”

But with a sinister twist. With low back pain, all too often the “joke” is all-too-serious, transmogrified into an ominous, hope-suffocating recommendation to give up, to give up on something you love:

“Doctor, my low back hurts when I play golf!”

“Well, then don’t play golf!”

To be clear, this mostly comes up with long-term back pain, where it is somewhat easier to forgive. But — in their ignorance of back pain and their conviction that backs are fragile — many health care professionals will treat your favourite activities as if they are a doomed way of life for you, so inevitably aggravating to your back pain that it is a foregone conclusion that you’ll have to give them up. Jogging, vigorous sports, golf, tennis, et cetera … whatever you miss doing, you are gravely told it might be time to let it go.

The weird part: this attitude can go so far wrong that your activities are actually blamed for your back pain, as if the activity itself were a diagnosis. In other words, your problem is that you’re trying to have too much fun.

Even worse, this kind of “diagnosis” is almost always packaged together with the insinuation that your problem may be in your head instead of your back. What a mess!

Please beware of any professional who counsels you to quit a favourite physical activity, assuming it’s not absurdly intense for your age. While a break may be appropriate in many cases (if you haven’t already been forced to do so), it is almost never ultimately necessary — or wise — to actually give up your fun forever. In most cases, recreational exercise is a part of the cure … not the cause!

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Could you have a “pinched” nerve? The nerve pinch myth

This was a full-page advertisement for Lyrica in National Geographic magazine back in the 2000s:

“Do you feel burning pain in your feet?” the ad asks. “Or uncomfortable tingling, numbness, stabbing, or shooting sensations? If so, you may have nerve pain.”

Yes, you might. But there’s an excellent chance you don’t. Science says so!

This is one of the most common and understandable concerns that people have about pain near any part of their spine, especially the top and the bottom of it, but it is also one of the most overblown of all common medical fears.

For all the reasons discussed above (and more below), the pain of a pinched nerve in the back is less common than most people think, both patients and pros. Many other common causes of pain and altered sensation routinely fool patients and professionals alike into suspecting “some kind of nerve problem.”

And a large percentage of that so-called neuropathy is probably coming from muscle, and is relatively treatable.

Nerves are notorious

I once had a nice older Italian client who would ask me, over and over again, in a thick, sing-songy Italian accent, “So, it’s-a nerve, eh?” No, I would say, it’s probably just a muscle knot, not a nerve. And then — as if we’d never discussed it — five minutes later he would ask again, “So, that’s-a nerve, eh?” He was obsessed with nerves!

Like everyone else is. Sometimes it seems to me as if modern civilization is still getting used to the whole idea of nerves. When people talk about their nerves, it’s like they’re talking about something just revealed by science early last year. They speak with some awe about something barely understood … and feared. Nerves! It could be my nerves!

Nerves just make people nervous. The whole idea of nerves gets people anxious. Could it be a nerve? people are likely to wonder of any puzzling pain. Is this a nerve problem? What if it’s a nerve? Is something pinching my nerve? Something must be pinching a nerve!

The idea of a pinched nerve root particularly is deeply embedded in the public consciousness, thanks to decades of excessive emphasis on the idea in both mainstream and alternative medicine. The fraudulent marketing of anticonvulsants for neuropathy is the most spectacular and fresh example, but chiropractors have promoted this kind of thinking consistently for over a century.261 And now many people think they can hardly get out of bed in the morning without pinching a nerve root.

Nerve roots actually have a lot of wiggle room

All the nerve fear is peculiar, because it’s amazingly difficult to actually pinch most nerves in the back. And, even when you do, they are usually surprisingly unbothered by it.

In the back, we’re speaking mainly of nerve roots, where they exit the spine. In general, nerve roots have generous “wiggle room.” For instance, in the lumbar spine, the holes between the vertebrae that the nerve roots pass through can be more than a couple centimetres at their widest, while the nerve roots themselves are only about 3-4mm thick.262 If you stretch or compress the spine, the holes do change size a little — as much as 70–130% in the looser neck joints,263 a little less in the low back.264 But, even at their most compressed, there’s still plenty of room.

Schematic of nerve root wiggle room

On the left are the approximate proportions of a healthy nerve root & the hole it passes through (intervertebral foramen). When the spine is pulled or compressed, the holes get a little larger or smaller, as shown on the right … but there’s still lots of nerve root room. Click to embiggen.

There’s so much space for nerve roots that significant arthritic deformation and even major derangements of the spine (dislocations, spinal stenosis) can fail to actually cause trouble.265266 Once again I invoke the example of a patient of mine with a severe lumbar dislocation … and no symptoms at all, not even symptoms of pinched nerve roots. Her nerves seemed to be fine, even in an anatomical situation most people would assume to be extremely dangerous.

X-Factors: pinching in itself is not enough to cause trouble (seriously)

The idea of nerve pain is almost synonymous with nerve root pinching … but it shouldn’t be. Physical impingement is only part of the equation, and maybe it’s not even the biggest part. There are other factors.

Like oxygen. You need it. Your nerves need it. And both you and your nerves get cranky without it.

Fun fact: healthy, fully oxygenated nerves can be pinched firmly without causing pain, but oxygen-starved nerves are sensitive.267268269270 Related: there’s also some good evidence that radiculopathy has more to do with blood supply than mechanical impingement.271

And why would blood supply to a nerve root be impinged? According to Jayson, “Vascular damage and fibrosis are common within the vertebral canal and intervertebral foramen.” Especially after surgery! But not only after surgery. The delicate capillaries around nerve roots seem to degenerate just like joints get arthritic, and that process is probably accelerated by biological factors like autoimmune disease, cardiovascular disease, and chronic low grade inflammation … which are in turn affected by diet, fitness, stress, sleep, etc.

Despite the fact that nerve-pinch pain is a thing, it’s clear that tissue health is probably the more important factor. The vulnerability of the nerve before it’s pinched is probably more important than the fact that it’s being pinched, or how hard. And how vulnerable the nerves are may be affected by factors that have nothing whatsoever to do with your back. Like your genes!

Nerve pain and sensitization

There are several mechanisms by which nerves can become pathologically over-sensitive after an initial insult, causing the pain to drag on and on. For a long time, no one had any idea why this happened to some people and not others, and it really does seem to be a binary phenomenon: either it happens or it doesn’t. Unfortunately, one likely explanation was identified in 2010: genetics.272 That is not great news, but it is interesting and at least a little bit useful.

So chronic pain could be due to on-going irritation of nerve tissue, but it could also be entirely due to a malfunction of the sensory equipment itself. A fascinating possibility (and a rather bleak one).

The point: be wary of therapeutic wild goose chases looking for mechanical causes of pain. Neuropathy is definitely not just about pinching. The extended suffering could be caused by continuing irritation of a nerve root, or it could be entirely due to a malfunction of the sensory equipment itself.

The relationship to trigger points

If nerve pain is more about biological nerve vulnerability and poor health of tissue around them than physical irritation, what does that say about the role of trigger points? Maybe they are just another symptom of poor tissue health, or could they be a form of “poor tissue health” themselves. Could a nerve passing through/near muscle tissue rotten with trigger points be affected by that? I’m not sure how plausible it is, but it’s not inconceivable.

Maybe this is why treating trigger points sometimes seems to alleviate actual neuropathy.273 If so, it’s yet another way that a back problem that seemingly isn’t about muscle may nevertheless be helped by treating muscle. In this articulate passage, Clair Davies, author of The Trigger Point Therapy Workbook (my review), discussed patterns he observed in private practice as a massage therapist. My experience has been similar over the years …

Interestingly, almost all the people who came to me had some kind of back pain along with whatever other pain complaint they had. Their previous treatments for back pain had always focused on the spine. I heard about injections of papaya or cortisone. People had usually been told they had arthritis or bad disks, or that their cartilage had been worn away. They’d been shown X-rays [or MRIs! — PI] that purported to prove it. Some had already had surgery, and frequently had as much pain after surgery as before. Typically, the surgeon’s last word was always that he was sorry but he’d done all he could. Then he’d renew their prescription for painkillers and dump them off on a physical therapist. I heard these stories over and over again. And over and over, I found that trigger point therapy gave them the relief they’d been seeking for so long. Had trigger points been the problem in the first place? Arthritis? Bad disks? In Travell and Simons’ Myofascial Pain and Dysfunction, I had read that you can have herniated discs and arthritis of the spine and still find that myofascial trigger points are the primary cause of your back pain.

The trigger point therapy workbook, by Clair Davies, p. 13

Comparison of Nerve Pain and Trigger Point Pain
Nerve Pain Trigger Point Pain
often causes tingling and pins and needles almost never causes pins and needles
electrical, zappy, hot, burning deep, aching, stabbing
often causes true numbness may cause a “dead” or “heavy” feeling, but you will still be able to feel light touch on the skin
very specific pattern/locations sometimes quite variable
injured nerves tend to produce continuous symptoms, or symptoms that occur predictably in response to a certain movement or positiontrigger point pain, while it certainly can respond to position and movement, is usually more variable and unpredictable
only a few nerves in the body are commonly hurt although more common in certain areas, trigger point pain also routinely occurs everywhere else

I will explore cluneal nerve entrapment in other chapters, an example of a neuropathy that is more likely to masquerade as trigger points than other neuropathies, but which also has clear symptoms of nerve botheration. And in the next section, an example of the reverse: pain that seemed a lot like neuropathy but probably was not.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Case study: nerve pain completely resolved by massage

I once helped with a young woman who had “sciatica” — the mother of all nerve pinches. The sciatic nerve is the biggest single peripheral nerve in the human body. Allegedly, either her sciatic nerve, or one of the lumbar nerve roots it emerges from, was being pinched and sending hot zaps of pain down her leg.

That can happen, so it was a plausible diagnosis, and she came to me with it already accepted. She also had some tingling in her feet (much like in the magazine advertisement). The description of her symptoms did, indeed, sound a lot like nerve distress. On the face of it, it was likely that her sciatic nerve was irritated: true sciatica, a genuine neuropathy. Although nerve pain isn’t nearly as common as trigger point pain, it certainly can happen.

A couple things didn’t add up, though. For instance, she had no numbness at all — no dead patches of skin, which are highly characteristic of true nerve impingement. Instead, she had widespread “dead heaviness” in her leg, a different kind of numb feeling that is much more closely associated with trigger points than nerve pinches — and a lot more common.

I quizzed her carefully about the quality of her pain. She assured me it was “zappy” and “electrical” … just as you would expect of nerve pain, not knots. Yet something didn’t seem quite right. I couldn’t shake the impression that she was interpreting non-neurological pain as an electrical mainly due to her strong belief that she had a nerve problem. When you think a pain is nervy, you’re going to interpret, feel and describe it in nervy terms. Pain quality is an extremely flexible concept.

So I did some experimenting, and clinched the case:

This young woman’s “nerve” pain could be vividly reproduced by pressing on muscle knots that were nowhere close to any nerve tissue. Pressing on the side of her hip, on a gluteus medius trigger point several centimetres away from the sciatic nerve, she reported the same “electrical” pain flowing down her leg, even producing the weird, tingling sensations in her foot.

That largely eliminated a diagnosis of sciatic nerve impingement,274 and the symptoms were fairly easy to relieve. No more neuropathy.

A more likely story

In spite of spending most of my career trying to explain to people that pain has many possible causes, and that muscle pain is particularly common and sneaky, I was surprising myself — fooled, really.

The symptoms really did seem neuropathic to me at first. But the evidence was hard to argue with, and — in retrospect — I realized that I had been sucked in by “nerve anxiety” myself. In fact, her symptoms were strongly consistent with a diagnosis of muscle pain.

The only unusual thing about her case was that her muscle knots produced referred pain that was more similar to nerve pain than usual, and even that may have been a by-product of my leading questions when I still assumed it was probably a neuropathy. That is, I may have accidentally encouraged the use of terms like “electrical” by basically suggesting them to her: “Is it an ‘electrical’ sensation?” If allowed to describe her pain in her own words, she might have done so in a less “nervy” way.

I suspect that muscle knots are routinely doing this, fooling patients and professionals alike. Painful trigger points are definitely more common than neuropathy, and at least some of those muscle knots feel enough like nerve pain that they are easily mistaken for the more familiar bogeyman.

The take-home message of this section: do not underestimate the power of trigger points to cause pain that seems like a nerve pinch.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The role of true nerve problems in low back pain

Sometimes, when it feels like you really have nerve pain, it’s because you really have nerve pain! But so what?

I argued above that nerve pinches are actually not a common cause of back pain: they are hard to pinch in the first place, and often not bothered by it when they are.

But “you’re not paranoid if they’re really after you.” Sometimes, when it feels like you really have nerve pain, it’s because you really have nerve pain (neuropathy, one of the basic types of pain).

Slightly more than one in ten people (not just back pain patients, but everyone) have nerve root trouble that can be confirmed electrodiagnostically.275 That may be a lower number than people expect, but impaired nerve root function is not exactly rare.

Here’s a fascinating for-instance: sometimes the sacrum is fused to the lowest lumbar vertebra by a bridge of bone, creating a “transitional vertebra” — a vertebra that can’t decide if it’s lumbar or sacral. They are the most common congenital anomaly of the low back, found in a whopping 7% of the population (at least, possibly much more) … and they probably do cause some trouble, the pesky little buggers.276 Back pain of this type is called “Bertolotti’s syndrome.” Specifically, nerve root compression occurs in about 13% of people with a transitional vertebra, and about 70% of those are symptomatic.277 This is a pitch perfect example of how structural issues can be a source of trouble … but there is also evidence that it’s not as much of a problem as one might think: it’s not much more common in people with pain, maybe even no more common at all.278 So it does cause some trouble, but not as much as expected and assumed.

This is probably the case for most seemingly obvious causes of nerve root trouble: they just aren’t as “mechanical” and simple as they seem. Nothing in biology ever is.

Disc herniations are probably the most common of the “obvious” causes of nerve root trouble, and there are a variety of other relatively rare possibilities.

So if you suspect that your back pain is the tip of a neuropathic iceberg, it’s important to be able to work towards confirmation of that suspicion.

The three main kind of back-related neuropathy

The Big Three types of neuropathy are all about location, location, location. There are three locations where nerve tissue can be bothered, each of them associated with its own “pathy.”

  • spinal cord → myelopathy
  • nerve roots → radiculopathy
  • nerve “branches”279 (peripheral nerves) → peripheral neuropathy

Radiculopathy is the disease of a troubled spinal nerve root. A pair of nerve roots splits off from the spinal cord at each vertebra, and exits through big holes on each side of the spine. They are about a centimetre or two long. The nerve roots quickly split into major nerve trunks, which rapidly split into smaller nerves branching out into the body. Bothering nerve roots usually causes pain, tingling, numbness, and weakness throughout the tissue those nerves penetrate, in a roughly “dermatomal” pattern (see below). “Radicular” pain (the pain of radiculopathy) is often like an electric shock.

Myelopathy usually refers to spinal cord impingement in the neck or upper back, causing complex problems “downstream.” In the lower back, the spinal cord ends fairly high up, where it then turns into a horsetail-like bundle of nerves, the cauda equina.280 Impingement of that is the same idea as cervical myelopathy in principle, but it’s usually clinically simpler (because there’s so much less downstream of it). Symptoms mostly only occur only when the spinal canal is narrowed quite a bit by significant degeneration, deformity, or injury. Narrowing of the spinal canal is spinal stenosis, and fairly common in older people. Interestingly, impingement does not lead inevitably to symptoms: as with any other nerve tissue, the cauda equina is not necessarily vulnerable to a bit of squeezing. “It depends.” So that’s nice to know.

Peripheral neuropathy is trouble with nerves out in the body, away from the spine. The nerve roots of the lumbar spine rapidly divide into a complex web of nerve trunks, reaching out and down towards the glutes, called the “lumbar plexus.” If the plexus gets into trouble, that’s a “peripheral” neuropathy (even though it’s happening very close to the spine). Despite that proximity, these injuries are rare (they are well protected) and rarely have anything to do with chronic back pain: they cause symptoms mostly in the buttocks and leg (sciatica).

Neuropathic symptoms

Both radiculopathy and myelopathy can cause chronic back pain! Neither problem is common, and they definitely don’t always cause back pain, but it is one of the standard symptoms. Exactly how often they cause back pain is unknown. And then they also usually cause symptoms “downstream” in the tissues affected by those nerves (practically anywhere in the lower body in the case of myelopathy). Really the only way to know whether your back pain is related to myelopathy or radiculopathy is if you have both back pain and other classic neuropathic symptoms. And so here’s an overview …

Classic symptoms of lumbar radiculopathy, which is insult to the nerve roots in the lower back (the bundles of nerves that emerge from holes in the spine):

  • pain spreading into the hips, legs and feet
  • especially “radicular” (zappy, shooting) pain (more on this below)
  • muscle weakness and/or numbness or tingling, especially in the calves, feet, and toes
  • feet slapping while walking, tripping

Classic symptoms of lumbar myelopathy and/or acute myelopathy (cauda equina syndrome):281

  • back pain
  • pain and/or weakness in either leg, but especially both
  • pain that is “radicular”: hot, zappy, shooting pain (more on this below)
  • “saddle” numbness (where a saddle would touch: inner thighs, groin, central buttocks)
  • bladder, bowel, and sexual dysfunction (possibly quite subtle, such as difficult initiating urination)
  • an awkward gait

More about the scary symptoms of cauda equina syndrome … which turn out to be different and less scary that widely believed

Everyone should know that an abrupt attack of pain and weakness in both legs is potentially serious. Numb plumbing that doesn’t work right is worrisome! Just in case that wasn’t super obvious.

These are the signature symptoms of cauda equina syndrome. But CES doesn’t have exclusive rights to them.

Since forever, saddle numbness has been considered the big red flag for CES. But modern data now shows that it’s just about as common in people without CES!282 In fact, all CES symptoms are like this to some degree: they all occur in lots of people who have no detectable compression (with MRI). Every last one of them.

The only ones that are somewhat more trustworthy — the best red flags possible — are pain and weakness in both legs. To a lesser degree, difficulty starting to pee is also a little more common with CES. But even these more predictive symptoms still can and do occur in people who have no CES (that can be confirmed with imaging). Which is probably why many if not most CES-ish symptoms turn out to be false alarms — perhaps as high as 80% of them.284

So don’t panic! It’s amazing how weird and bad bodies can feel without anything being too terribly wrong.

This does beg a rather interesting question though: if numbness isn’t CES, what is going on? Great question!285

Fortunately, CES is rare, something a typical physician will supposedly see only once in their career — but it turns out that is a classic “zombie statistic,” living on long after it should have died, based on some dubious old data.286 It is indeed rare, but the truth is probably more like “a few cases” per career, or even “several” for providers who see more back pain cases.287 So this is kind of like warning you about being mauled by a bear — it can happen, but the overwhelming majority of humans will never have the displeasure.

The first rule of neuropathy club: symptoms are unreliable

Even the most dramatic and scary symptoms of CES are often phantoms and red herrings, which means that all bets are off. No symptom of any kind of lumbar neuropathy is a diagnostic slam dunk — not even close.

It is possible to have radiculopathy and myelopathy with no clear classic symptoms at all for long periods. And basically all of the symptoms also have other causes!

For instance, let’s consider the classic case of sciatica, where “pain below the knee” is the closest thing to a signature symptom of sciatica… but not every patient with sciatica has that symptom, and some with that symptom do not have sciatica.288 The symptoms just don’t tell the whole story.

And so ultimately this review is intended only to inspire further medical investigation. If you have chronic back pain and you have some other neurological symptoms … then it’s worth asking a doctor about it.

Radicular pain: what nerve root pinching feels like

Radiculopathy can cause very boring pain — just standard aches and pains — but it also often causes a very distinctive type of pain called radicular pain. (That’s basically just Latin for “root pain,” where radix = root.) Radicular pain makes a strong impression on people. It’s not boring.

  • It usually goes well into the legs, often past the knee and even as far down as the feet.
  • The quality is “electrical” pain that feels like it “zaps” or “shoots.” Sometimes the pain may have a somewhat more “hot piano wire” feeling than electrical. In any case, the pain comes in dramatic blasts along the thin line of a specific nerve — nothing like the more typical nagging aches and throbs of most low back pain.
  • Radicular pain is often associated with altered sensation as well:
    • True tactile numbness, in which you have reasonably well-defined patches of skin that cannot feel the prick of a pin.
    • Pins and needles — or “parasthesia,” which is Latin for altered sensation.
  • Radicular pain mostly occurs in patterns, following one of the dermatomes. Sort of — it’s less predictable than I was taught to expect. Most nerve root pinching in the back occurs at the bottom of the back, in the L4 to S1 dermatomes: a broad, spiralling tripe down the outside and back of the thigh, the front of the lower leg, and most of the feet. More about these patterns below.

None of that nonsense is normal for non-radicular low back pain, of course. Most low back pain has only minor effects on the legs, if any at all. Trigger points (muscle knots) in the back and hips often do spread aching and burning pains down the legs, and can definitely cause some confusion — but probably not vivid pain and tingling and numbness along a clearly defined path. And muscle pain can also make the legs feel heavy and dead, which is kind of like numbness — but not at all like the true lack of skin sensation that nerve impingement causes. The pins and needles and crawling prickles of neuropathy are especially distinctive.

More about radiculopathy symptoms: Nerve root trouble is felt in interesting patterns … kinda

Lots of data and instructions pass through a nerve root, coming to and from the tissues it supplies. Trouble with the root (radiculopathy) causes trouble throughout the distribution of the root’s downstream nerves, which spreads out in curious patterns: the dermatomes, the stripes of skin associated with each nerve root. Dermatomes spiral around the body like ribbons, down and away from the spine. Pain in a roughly “dermatomal” pattern is one of the most distinctive features of radiculopathy.

Only certain muscles are powered by each nerve root, too — myotomes — so trouble with a nerve root results in weakness of specific muscles.

So every nerve root is responsible for a couple of “tomes”: a skin-tome and a muscle-tome. A dermatome and a myotome.

A standard dermatome diagram.

A standard dermatome diagram showing the curious spiralling stripes of sensory innervation around the arms & legs. The reality is much less tidy, with lots of fuzziness & overlap. A truthful dermatome diagram would look more like a child’s colouring book! Click to embiggen.

Pain, weakness, numbness, and tingling within a tome-zone are strong indicators that a particular nerve root is being squeezed or irritated. But pain and weird sensations usually don’t fill in the area of a dermatome precisely. Instead, they are filled in more like a child’s colouring book: lots of colouring outside the lines! A detailed dermatome mapping study back in 1998, in which brave subjects were subjected to deliberate provocation of their nerve roots — ouch! — finding strong trends but many exceptions: “symptoms were frequently provoked outside of the distribution of classic dermatomal maps.”289 In another study, less than half of patients with L5 nerve root compression identified symptoms in the L5 dermatome.290 In yet another, only about a third was non-dermatomal; in the back, about two thirds.291 It’s all much less predictable than I was trained to expect.292

But why? Probably mainly because anatomy is messy. “Although there is a regular and orderly progression of innervation within each individual, it is likely that innervation patterns differ considerably between individuals.”293 That variability was confirmed in a cadaver study in 2000: no two corpses’ nerves were quite alike. Although nerve roots themselves were quite consistent, they had a rat’s nest of random small branches connecting them, like the tangled roots of a tree.294

Another wrinkle: even when the pain is in a dermatomal pattern, it may not “fill” the pattern, and pain is often experienced mainly in one section of the dermatome, often the most remote part. For instance, burning pain in the big toe, which lives in the dermatome for the bottom-most lumbar nerve root, L5. Most of the dermatome feels fine, but oh, that toe!

You can also get partial and mixed radicular pain involving portions and combinations of different tome-zones, by irritating not the root itself, but major branches off the root. The classic example is the sciatic nerve, which consists of several major branches all bundled together. This results in something like radicular pain … but not confined to nice tidy ‘tomes.

Despite all that, I want to emphasize that the patterns are still mostly reliable: for instance, you don’t have to worry about finding the symptoms of a C6 radiculopathy way over in the C4 dermatome. But adjacent dermatomes likely do overlap more than most professionals think. And this stuff is why neurologists get paid the big bucks. Even the simplified concepts are difficult.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The strange case of scoliosis

Scoliosis is right out of the X-files of musculoskeletal medicine: an unexplained anatomical defect that gets multiplied over the many joints of the spine, producing an overall sideways curvature that is much greater than the glitch in any one joint. It often starts in childhood, but it can strike at any age. It can stay subtle and stable for a lifetime, or it can progress relentlessly to extremes of deformity, or almost anything between.

Sometimes scoliosis has an obvious trigger, like the spine being pulled out of shape by the powerful spasms caused by some diseases, or as a reaction to a serious spinal injury (as it did in my wife). Scoliosis can even be induced! It happened to almost everyone who ever had ribs removed (eek!) to treat the consequences of tuberculosis.295 It’s almost like the spine is trying to squirm away from trouble.

But most scoliosis just comes out of the blue, and then often keeps coming (primary idiopathic degenerative scoliosis). Not only is this as weird as snake sneakers, it’s actually common. Spines do this lateral warping thing rather a lot. People with a little of it are simply everywhere: at least one out of three older women, maybe even half of them… and certainly at least one human of any kind in every full train car, busy coffee shop, or classroom.296

Severe cases are not common, but hard data on them is just as scarce as the patients. Horrifically spiralling spines have been staples of cabinets of medical curiosities for centuries, but that is an exotic fate, and most medical professionals will never see anyone like that.

Nearly everything about why spines do this thing remains a mystery, and a fine example of how primitive musculoskeletal health science still is. Delving into the nature of scoliosis is beyond the scope of this document — or apparently any document297 — but the condition can at least give us an interesting perspective on the uncertain relationship between spinal structure and pain. There is also a chapter on treatment for mild to moderate adult scoliosis later in the guide.

Vintage illustration of an extremely scoliotic spine, including rib cage and pelvis.

An illustration of extreme scoliosis, from a 1903 text on obstetrics by Cameron. Scoliosis this severe is life threatening & has basically nothing to do with common back pain or this book… but I can’t write about scoliosis without showing a vintage image of an extremely deformed spine for “colour.” It’s in my contract, I think.

Does scoliosis cause back pain?

Is scoliosis a significant factor in back pain? That’s what this chapter is about, and the answer seems obvious.

It isn’t actually obvious, of course.

It seems that scoliosis must surely increase the stress on spinal tissues — how can it not? And maybe that can add up to a problem over time. Perhaps 60% of scoliotic teens have back pain, and if the scoliosis worse, so is the pain.298 (I have been unable to find stats on pain in adult scoliosis.) But that’s roughly the strongest reported link. Other data suggests that pain in scoliosis patients is quite bit less common than 60%, maybe only 30% over a lifetime299 — plenty of painful scoliosis, but even more that’s painless.

The link between back pain and scoliosis so murky that legendary spine expert Dr. Alf Nachemson wrote, in 1979, “From available long-term follow-up studies of untreated scoliosis, there seems to be minimal risk of disabling back pain in adult patients who have lumbar curves.”300 And nothing’s really changed, because the world of back pain research moves like molasses: in 2021, Yuan et al pointed out that there is “no consensus as to whether non-specific low back pain in scoliosis patients is related to scoliosis per se or is just a normal symptom that could happen in anyone.”301 Still.

If we still can’t agree on that, it’s unlikely that scoliosis is all that painful. If it was, there would be a consensus!

Modern evidence certainly does show some risk, but it’s just not the slam dunk of a link most people think it has to be. The pain is notably not clearly worse than other kinds of back pain (a bit different, causing more groin and thigh pain).302 There are so many other factors in back pain that are not “stress on spinal tissues,” and back pain in the scoliotic is indeed clearly linked to the same squishy factors (insomnia, depression) that contribute to any kind of back pain. So the scoliosis may not actually be the main problem in the scoliotic patients who do have pain.

It is particularly silly to blame severe chronic low back pain on a minor scoliosis, but this happens more or less constantly. If you have minor scoliosis, good luck getting out of a massage appointment without being told “well, there’s your problem.” Even when there is minor scoliosis and pain, the main clinical question there should be why is minor scoliosis a problem? Because it usually isn’t. So there’s probably something else going on.

Scoliosis the terrible: Becky’s back

Extremes are instructive. Becky was a patient of mine in the 2000s, and while her back wasn’t quite eligible for any cabinets of medical curiosities… it was pushing it. He spine began literally spiralling out of control when she was quite young, and by the time she was twelve it was rotated to about 70˚ — a very severe scoliosis. Her internal organs were being slowly crushed, and radical surgical intervention was required to save her life.303

Severe scoliosis, but minimal low back pain

My former client Becky has intervertebral joints rotated to about 40˚, despite having large steel braces surgically implanted at the age of 12. If minor scoliosis was a significant cause of low back pain, then Becky should be crippled — but in the years that I knew here, she was more fit & active than I was.

Even after implanting large stainless steel braces in her back — followed by a year of difficult recovery — the powerful process of scoliosis continued to bend Becky’s back and the titanium rods. But over the years the metal was slowly winning the wrestling match, and by 2010 she was rotated to “only” about 40˚. (I have lost contact with Becky in the years since, though I hope I will be able to find her eventually and update her story here.)

That’s all quite dramatic, but it’s not actually the amazing thing. What’s most remarkable about Becky’s case is that she was not a back pain patient. Not really. Not the kind of low back pain patient that buys this guide!

Like the client described earlier with the drastic spondylolisthesis — one vertebrae practically falling off the one below it — Becky was incredibly fit and surprisingly pain-free, considering what was going on in her back. She did have some back pain, and occasionally it was even strong. But she was nevertheless unmistakably better off than most of the low back pain patients I have ever known. Her pain was relatively minor. She played ultimate and other vigorous sports, and did not feel or act fragile.304 She did not even hire me primarily to help with her back (although it was a strong “as long as I’m here” bonus reason).

By contrast, most people with serious and stubborn low back pain — the target market for this guide — are often “flattened” by nasty episodes of pain, or suffer almost unbearable constant nagging pain, like a toothache in the back.

Becky’s back is about as bad as a back can get structurally. But she probably had less back pain than many of you do right now, with spines that look perfect by comparison. Weird, isn’t it?

As with all such stories, the general explanation is almost certainly that pain is more biological than mechanical and requires a combination of factors, including non-obvious ones. Even many super obvious spinal problems are routinely neither necessary nor sufficient for pain.

Minor and imaginative scoliosis as a scapegoat

Legitimate but clearly minor scoliosis is routinely blamed for back pain. Some therapists automatically screen for scoliosis, and attribute any back pain the patient has to even the most subtle curvatures of the spine.

This just doesn’t add up. I have seen numerous cases of severe scoliosis over the years — none as dramatic as Becky’s, but several that were obviously deforming. All of those patients had “annoying” low back pain and stiffness, but not one had a serious low back pain problem.

And so, like all the other common structural diagnoses, scoliosis fails a simple test of logic: if a minor scoliosis is supposedly responsible for severe low back pain, then why are patients like Becky still able to walk? Shouldn’t they be crippled?

As if it weren’t bad enough that therapists tend to exaggerate the clinical importance of mild scoliosis in low back pain, they often diagnose scoliosis where there is simply none at all.

Many, many times in my hands-on career I was told “my other therapist says I have scoliosis,” but I could only see it if I squinted and used my imagination. Although technically any spiralling of the vertebrae is scoliotic, subtle cases just don’t qualify for the diagnosis — “the incidence of significant scoliosis is low” write the authors of a large 1999 study of scoliosis screening.305 They also think a common screening test for scoliosis (a test I’ve used many times myself) is probably deeply flawed.

Low back pain patients have enough to worry about without being told that they have scoliosis when it probably wouldn’t be clinically important even if they actually had it. Worse, they may not have scoliosis at all, or it’s so mild that you could argue about whether it’s there.

Just as scoliosis is unjustly blamed for much more back pain than it probably causes, many therapists also bizarrely claim to be able to cure scoliosis — to straighten the twisted spine. This will be debunked further along in the tutorial.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Back pain and sneezing

There is a connection between sneezes and some back pain, believe it or not. What does it mean when sneezes hurt?

Sneezes violently contract many of the muscles of the trunk and also dramatically compress the abdominal contents.  Pains that occur only or mainly with sneezes suggest a problem that is particularly sensitive to abdominal pressure, and/or a problem that is relatively minor, noticed only during the intensity of a sneeze.

Any pain easily triggered by spikes in abdominal pressure should be checked with a doctor. There are several possibilities. Many are no big deal. Some are a bigger deal. You may be able to recognize a big-deal problem by looking for the symptom in non-sneeze situations. For instance, coughing is less intense than sneezing, but still increases abdominal pressure: if you notice the pain when coughing as well as sneezing, it indicates a greater sensitivity to abdominal pressure. Straining on the toilet is another good example, and you can simulate that strain simply by “blowing on your thumb.”306 If these milder pressures duplicate your symptom, take note: something in your abdomen does not like relatively minor increases in pressure! That is worth running by your doctor.

On the other hand, the more this pressure-sensitive symptom resembles your familiar back pain, the less worried you need to be.

Because sneezing involves powerful contractions of muscles throughout the trunk, irritated muscles or other anatomical structures could be disturbed by this.  Most often, grumpy anatomy will be obviously painful before the sneeze, and the sneeze simply spikes the pain momentarily.  However, it’s also possible for a problem to be silent until the sneeze, and then yell when the sneeze hits.

In some cases, the spot will then fall silent again, only briefly irritated.  That pattern tends to indicate a non-muscular source: something irritable that is only a problem when directly stressed.

More commonly, the sneeze marks the beginning of a new pain: a brief spike of sharper pain, followed by a new, persistent ache.  This pattern usually indicates an activated trigger point. The muscle is irritated by the sneeze and then continues to hurt. It’s quite surprising how many back pain symptoms originate with a sneeze! Personally, I’ve had at least one notable episode of back pain that began with a sneeze.

Since a sneeze affects virtually the entire trunk, there is no way to deduce from this issue which specific trigger points to focus on. You should simply hunt for trigger points in the tissues where the sneeze causes pain (and, of course, in muscles that refer pain to that area).

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Lions, tigers, and … back mice? The significance of lipomas

“Back mice” is a cute name for a very common thing: little fatty cysts that form in the low back.307 A lipoma is a lipid-filled cyst with the texture of gelatin. They can get fairly large, up to a few centimetres, and in very rare cases they get a lot bigger: mice elephants! Just do a Google image search for lipomas for some eye-popping examples. But nearly all lipomas are about the size of a kidney bean.

Lipomas usually feel separate from other tissue, a bit slippery, like a tiny, firm water balloon sliding around just under the skin.

Lipomas sometimes get labelled as back mice when they are symptomatic, which they usually aren’t. Once in a blue moon one of these little things will get a little irritated, usually because of minor mechanical pressure (against a chair back, say), or because it’s in a slightly awkward anatomical location (perhaps causing some minor nerve impingement308).

They are about as dangerous as actual mice, and are not very clinically interesting. Unless they are.

Not everyone agrees that back mice are trivial. There are case reports of lipomas that seemed to be more tightly linked to chronic back pain.309310 Dr. Harvey Austin wrote to tell me of his experience with them:

Early in my surgical career, I removed painful fatty nodules from the low back from eight patients and obtained relief in each. Personally, I have had multiple painful nodules removed nine times over 28 years. Each time my newly-occurring (and different) back pain either instantly disappeared or was markedly relieved.

My science-savvy readers will understand that a doctor’s experience is interesting-but-inconclusive. No matter how earnest, such reports are invariably filtered through beliefs and expectations and cannot be taken to the knowledge bank. Still, it’s a fair bit of anecdotal smoke, isn’t it?

It’s not clear why some back mice would be painful, but I’ll keep an open mind. (Dr. Austin believes that it’s simply a sub-type.311) But it’s almost certainly rare, and it’s possible — maybe even likely — that these case reports are simply wrong, and there is no such thing as a serious back mouse problem.312

Back mice might be of no clinical interest at all, except that they so often get mis-diagnosed, usually by patients. Patients often notice and fear them. They notice lipomas because they have back pain and go exploring, and lipomas are superficial and obvious and obviously abnormal.

So people think they are tumours. Which they are … technically. Just not the dangerous kind. Lipomas are basically just benign tumours.

And patients who have read anything about trigger points ever will usually mistake them for trigger points. And there’s nothing sadder than someone desperately trying to “release” a lipoma. Lipomas are one of the classic examples of common lumpy and bumpy things that fool beginners into thinking they’ve found a trigger point. Is there anything more sad than someone desperately trying to “release” a lipoma? I have lost count of the number of readers and patients over the years who have made this mistake, and this is a major motivation for finally adding this section to the book.

But a lipoma feels very different from how a trigger point feels. Trigger points feel embedded in the muscle, not something that can move around on top of the muscle.

For years I only knew of lipomas from my own work. But now I have one of very own back mice!

My pet theory about back mice

A pet mouse, in this case.

I suspect that back mice are more prevalent in a biological milieu of chronic low-grade inflammation and “inflammaging.” That phenomenon was thoroughly covered in the morning back pain chapter, but to briefly recap for readers who are jumping around: we all get a little more inflamed in a variety of ways as we age, for all kinds of reasons, some obvious, some mysterious, but generally ramping up over the years. The most prominent manifestations of this are the classic diseases of aging and poor fitness, but it’s now clear that there are many more subtle examples, such as the high prevalence of frozen shoulder in people most at risk for heart disease. I think it’s likely that there are a lot more examples yet to be discovered.

Like back mice, which definitely get more numerous with age. It seems plausible that back mice are both more common and more ferocious in people who are more inflamed. No one has tested this, of course. We’re way out in the weeds here. But it’s a solid hypothesis.

I am getting old and unhealthy. I have had a lot of serious problems with chronic pain since 2015. That drama is much bigger than this topic, but a huge increase in back pain has been part of my troubles, serious abnormal inflammation is a major suspected culprit, and I have developed three back mice — for the first time in my life — in the same period.

COINCIDENCE? I THINK NOT.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Should you get an MRI, X-ray, or other imaging?

MRI has come up several times already, always emphasizing that the tissue issues it reveals are not necessarily your problem. There is some repetition here, but I won’t re-hash the details. The point of this section is to sum up the role of MRI (and other back pain imaging) and provide some practical advice.

Short answer: yes, for severe, chronic, or otherwise ominous back pain. But there are major caveats, and a need for a longer answer.

When it comes to diagnosing most back pain, MRI machines are like Monty Python’s medical machinery that goes “bing.” For back pain, MRI and X-ray are medical machines that make false alarms.

They don’t always make false alarms, of course. MRI is a miracle technology, no doubt about it — the ability to get clear images of soft tissues deep inside the body is valuable, and extremely tempting for everyone involved.313 MRI can shine when it’s actually needed and done well.

But the sad truth is that it’s not needed anywhere near as much as it is actually used, and it’s especially not needed for most low back pain. And not doing it “well”? That’s just normal: radiology reports are usually written without clinical context, and frequently presented to the patient like diagnoses. But low back pain experts have long understood that you simply cannot reliably diagnose low back pain with MRI or with X-ray in isolation — and trying to do so reliably raises false alarms that actually do harm.314 Premature MRI is actually often worse than useless.

Wise use of MRI is great (and I’ll finish with a summary of best practices). But the common overuse and abuse of MRI is a disaster of over-medicalization.

The over-imaging debacle is official (and it has been for years)

The lameness of MRI point was pointed out firmly by the American College of Physicians and the American Pain Society in their 2007 guidelines for the management of low back pain. Bizarrely, that message has not yet reached many therapists and doctors. Scientific journals are still publishing (and re-publishing) guidelines like these — perhaps eventually the message will get through!

The most important recommendation was that doctors should avoid giving people X-rays and MRI unless the clinical situation is really rather bad, such as severe and persistent neurological symptoms. The authors labelled this recommendation “strong,” and believe the evidence to support it is “moderate.”

I think that the recommendation should now be “stronger than strong, very strong, really extremely strong,” and the evidence supporting it (already all reviewed above) can be considered “bulletproof.”

That image shows the full history of that persons life. It shows the fresh wounds that may be related to their current pains, but it also shows all of the scars that they’ve accumulated over the years. So in showing the wounds and scars, no radioloist should be giving opinons as to what features correlate with pain until they’ve seen the patient and assessed them and understood their pains. This is a general flaw in medicine. I don’t believe radiologists should write a report without the context of the pain that the person presents with.

Stuart McGill, interviewed by John Childs for Evidence In Motion podcast (2018)

The good news: it’s not actually hard to do MRI right (or at least a lot better)

Doing it right mainly means taking fancy pictures of our insides only when it’s actually needed — that is, when there are ominous signs and symptoms, or significant chronicity.

But it also means using better tools for the job as needed, especially positional and dynamic imaging, which can show problems that just won’t turn up on garden variety scans. And metabolic scans, which show inflammatory hot spots, which can highlight a joint that’s in trouble right now (versus one that merely has scars from an incident many year ago). Some of these imaging techniques have greater risks, and so the need must be even greater to justify them — but sometimes these are the only tools that will get the job done.

Most importantly, it means that imaging should be always be interpreted in clinical context. It’s not a “finding” until it connects in some way with a person’s case.

What’s the big deal? Why is it so important to actually avoid using X-ray and MRI to diagnose back pain?

No one wants a false alarm, but what’s the big deal about a few diagnostic red herrings? It’s a worse problem than you might think in (at least) two major ways …

First, you can’t un-see an MRI. X-rays and MRIs genuinely spook people! If you see something that seems like a problem on a scan, complete reassurance is nearly impossible, no matter how expertly and convincingly someone explains why it’s not actually a problem.

Second, imaging often just fails to clarify the situation, or it actually muddies the diagnostic waters. A mountain of scientific evidence clearly suggests that back pain correlates really, really badly with these test results. Many people with no pain have all kinds of things “wrong” with their backs, and vice versa. Many problems revealed by scans that seem like “obvious” problems are not. And so the diagnosis and treatment often goes spinning off in the wrong direction. This is a major part of the reason why there are such scary statistics about the economic costs of back pain.

There are exceptions — sometimes imaging finds something important — and that’s why these tests can be appropriate for some kinds of severe and persistent low back pain. But it’s just a generally lousy way to try to figure out why your back hurts.

More good news: patients can do a lot about this

Despite all the science and warnings, it is still routine for me to hear from readers who have been X-rayed by their chiropractors and MRI-ed by their doctors in the early stages of back pain. And it’s still routine for those people to be told that what was found on their MRI is conclusive evidence of the cause of their back pain. I have almost never heard of a patient who was told that signs of typical degeneration might be meaningless, even though that’s what every patient should be told.

Take matters into your own hands.

Ideally, better imaging is going to need better doctors and a better system, of course. But improvements can be surprisingly patient driven. With just a little bit of education, patients can…

  • Cheerfully refuse premature imaging! If you get back pain, and someone tries to beam rays through you prematurely, just say, “Thanks, but no thanks. The American Pain Society says it isn’t necessary unless I can’t feel my legs.”
  • Politely ask if other imaging options are appropriate. For instance, if you are being told that no cause for your back pain can be detected, but you have a clearly repeatable pain with a specific movement or position, ask if — perhaps, just maybe — you should be imaged in that position, or moving through it.
  • Privately resolve to take radiology reports with a huge grain of salt, regardless of what the doctors say. You should also try to ask your doctor for a more nuanced interpretation of the results in clinical context, but that’s a lot trickier — many doctors won’t have any better idea of how to do that than you do yourself! The main thing is just to maintain a strong healthy skepticism.

The pros and cons of MRI for back pain (from an educated doctor’s perspective)

This is brilliant:

Comic by Patrick Lyons of Coogee Bay Physiotherapy.

This is a great comic, but not every reader is going to fully appreciate the humour in the doctor’s thoughts, so I’ll elaborate a bit:

  • What’s a “bottom up understanding of back pain,” and why’s that bad? It’s the idea that back pain comes primarily from backs (bottom up), when in fact we have really strong evidence that back pain severity and chronicity is powerfully tuned by the brain (top down).
  • Greater disability scores associated with MRI utilization.” One of the most common ways of measuring the badness of back pain is “disability” as determined by a very carefully designed questionnaire. And disability gets worse (higher scores) when MRI is involved in the assessment of back pain, probably because it “medicalizes” and dramatizes. This is a nocebo effect (opposite of placebo). Basically, looking for things wrong with people’s spines makes people fear their spines, which ironically leads to hypervigilance, sensitization, and disability.
  • Reduced sense of well-being following exposure to MRI.” Very similar to the previous item! “Well-being” can be high even when you have a bunch of back pain … or it can be low. When back pain is over-medicalized — too much fancy diagnosis, scary treatment options bandied about — people feel worse about their situation. More worried!

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Ribs and back pain

I debated for a bit whether or not rib pathology belongs in a book about back pain, but I decided it was a no-brainer: it’s a near perfect example of an obscure but specific issue that is at the root of some cases of back pain. There’s even some cheap, safe, reasonable self-treatment options to try. Plenty of rib pain has nothing to do with the back at all, of course. Many disorders of the ribs cause discomfort only or mainly in chest, abdomen, or flank (and even lower abdominal and groin pain are possible).

But the discomfort does often extends to the back. It might even dominate it.

Rib pain also fits strongly with a major theme of this back pain book: muscle pain. One of the major ways that rib trouble turns into back trouble is probably the discomfort of exhausted muscle trying to stabilize those sloppy lower ribs.

Many patients will have no idea of the potential for ribs to cause back pain, and plenty of professionals will be just as surprised by the extent of it — because until 2023, the variability and instability of the lower ribs was simply unknown to science, let alone frontline clinicians. Ribcage anatomy is still advancing, well into the 21st Century.

Introducing slipped rib syndrome

The lower ribs are somewhat unstable, and they can slide under a neighbour and pinch an intercostal nerve (among other discomforts). This is known as slipped rib syndrome.

As I started to write about SRS, an old friend was coincidentally suffering through a nasty case of it. He had a classic and severe blaze of intense pain around one side of his lower ribcage and extending all the way into his low back. It had started suddenly with no apparent provocation a couple weeks earlier, as I was beginning my research, and it made his life hell for a few days:

“I was in so much pain I couldn’t sleep, couldn’t breathe properly, couldn’t lie down or sit, couldn’t really do anything.” So he just sweated and squirmed his way through. “It’s been backing off this week, but it’s still bad.”

This was a new experience for him, but he has a form of muscular dystrophy that has already given him a lot of back trouble, and it probably increases the risk of this rib nonsense (due to weakness of the trunk musculature). But there’s also a much more common risk factor that no doctor or physical therapist would have known about … because no one could have known about it without reading a recent study of the anatomy of the ribcage.

You’d think ribcage anatomy would be settled science, but nope! And that is what I was writing about: not only is the lower ribcage an anatomically chaotic area, but specifically there are far more floating ribs down there than we see in the textbooks … and they are slippery. It’s new anatomy that matters.

“The word ‘normal’ is probably an inappropriate word to apply to the human body.”

Dr. Ian Griffiths

Rib research that matters: a lot more floating ribs than anyone thought possible

It’s getting routine for scientists to personally tell me about their research, which is delightful. Dr. Evert Eriksson, Trauma Medical Director at the Medical University of South Carolina, read my article about anatomical variation — about all the ways that anatomy differs from one person to the next — and he thought, I should tell this guy about my rib study! Bet he’ll like that. And he did tell me, and I did like it. Very much.

One could hardly ask for a better example of an interesting and problematic body oddity. What Laswi et al. found, studying 8th, 9th, and 10th ribs in forty cadavers, was a lot more free 9th tips, and entire floating 10th ribs, than they were supposed to find:315

  1. A whopping 59% of forty cadavers had three fully floating ribs — 10th, 11th, and 12th — rather than the officially standard pair.

  2. And 86% of 9th ribs had a free tip, even though they were not otherwise true floating ribs. The 9th was always (100%) attached to the 8th by cartilage, but … messily, and most of their tips were free floating. So the 9th is kind of a hybrid (partly a “false” rib, partly a floater).

(Rib types: True ribs are directly joined to the sternum; false ribs are indirectly joined; floating ribs are not attached to the sternum at all.)

The general theme here is quite a lot more costal margin hypermobility and variability than expected. Even if their findings were exaggerated,316 they would still be important. The authors reasonably conclude that “the 9th and 10th ribs are not invariably attached to the costal margin as described in anatomy books.” Consistent with all that floating and messy looseness of the lower ribs, Laswi et al. also found plenty of subluxations and free and “upwardly hooked tips” — all potential sources of discomfort and pain, maybe downright likely sources. If free rib tips are more common than anyone realized? Well, then so are their problems.

The new normal: three floating ribs, not two

All of Laswi et al.’s findings may be anatomically significant, but the floating tenths is a bit of bombshell. The paper is respectably calm and professional about it. I can make more of a fuss about it here. This is kind of a big, fascinating deal.

No one should be surprised that there are a few 10th ribs floating around out there — anatomical quirks are legion317 — but more than half? That’s the surprise. That’s not a “quirk”! If true, it clashes with the textbooks, and redefines “normal.”

Redefining normal is tight. (That’s a reference to a running gag on my favourite comedic movie review channel, “Pitch Meeting.”) It’s really neat when research challenges extremely long-term assumptions about anatomy!

Look at most model skeletons or anatomical diagrams and they will show just two floating ribs. I checked several standard sources, and they all described only an 11th and 12th floating rib, with the occasional mention of “rare” 10th floaters. Above the 10th, the 9th rib is always depicted as a nice tidy false rib, merging with the cartilage that links it to the sternum.318 Extra floating ribs are often said to be common in the Japanese, a claim mentioned all over the internet, but supported with only one dubious old citation that I have not been able to find.319320 Wikipedia’s rib cage page mentions floater variation, but only barely — 10th floaters occur “sometimes” in “several ethnic groups, most significantly the Japanese” — and citing just a textbook (not the Japanese study). But they do show the original, vintage version of Gray’s ribcage … which does include three floaters! Interesting.

Vintage anatomical diagram of the back of a ribcage from the classic Gray’s Anatomy text, which still the only decent source of public domain anatomical illustrations. This diagram shows only the lower half the ribcage, and has my labelling, pointing out the tips of three floating ribs, not just two, which are also highlighted in red to suggest some potential for discomfort.

Most anatomical diagrams of the ribcage show only two floating ribs — as is normal. There are probably exceptions, but this is the only one I know of: the original, vintage Gray’s Anatomy, plate 112. Notice how sharp they look! Eek!

But in all the modern renderings I checked, both the 9th and the 10th are tidy false ribs, with no free tips. The reality as reported by Laswi et al. is far messier and looser, with way more 9th and 10th rib tips roaming free than anyone has ever known.

I have to turn in my “freak” card: my weird ribs aren’t so exotic after all!

Bonus rib messiness (not covered by Laswi et al.): there’s sometimes even an extra floating rib: the 13th!

Several years ago, I discovered that I have both a floating 10th and a stubby little floating 13th. I marvelled at how I’d missed those oddities for so many years — but who counts their floating ribs?! Not even me, not even with a long history of weird rib pain and a keen interest in anatomy. It simply did not occur to me.

Based on conventional anatomy, I assumed that my floating 10th was relatively rare. But with the 60% incidence identified by Dr. Eriksson’s group, a floating 10th is not just a quirk … it’s actually how the spine models should be built! It’s what should be in the texts! 🤯 Even if another study found “just” 40%, it’s clear that this anatomical bug/feature is actually much more common than red hair, hazel eyes, or having a tongue long enough to touch the tip of your nose. There are clearly a lot of floating ribs out there — probably a lot more than most physical therapists ever imagined.

I guess I have to turn in my “freak” membership card, because a 10th floater is less exotic than having freckles.

But I have had many of the symptoms of “slipped rib syndrome” off and on for my entire life, gradually worsening over the years. Isn’t “normal” anatomy fun? It’s time to talk about pathology…

“59% of us float down here. You might float, too.”

Pennywise, AKA "It" (Stephen King)

What could possibly go wrong? More floaters = more pathology

Subluxations, slipped rib syndrome, and hooked tips, that’s what. ☹️ Slipped rib syndrome is the catch-all term for a variety of problems related to the looseness of the lower ribs and their stabby floating tips.321 (This is very similar to Tietze syndrome and costochondritis, but not quite the same thing.322)

These lower ribs aren’t exactly the most stable anatomy to begin with, and the floaters are especially wibbly-wobbly. The lower ribs are more chaotically and loosely attached to each other and the costal margin than conventional anatomy has ever suggested, probably even in healthy young people — and age no doubt makes it worse as their attachments to each other and to the costal margin loosen and break, the ribs sliding under and over each other, and the tips in particular making trouble by poking intercostal nerves.

The end result for many patients is back pain, flank pain, some abdominal, and occasionally even groin pain: “a complex pain response,” as Laswi et al. put it. Unsurprisingly, symptoms are triggered by practically any movement or position of the trunk. Almost any combination of consistency, chronicity, and severity seems to be possible, which makes diagnosis quite challenging. Some people experience dramatic episodes of severe pain, which may last minutes or hours or days, but they may be largely pain-free between episodes — or not! Others may live with milder and more erratic symptoms for many years, never knowing what’s going on.

The symptoms of slipped rib syndrome are as variable as the anatomy, because the pathogenesis equation is freakishly complex. There are at least four ways that these loose ribs can cause trouble:

  • Subluxation (partial dislocation) is the most obvious and distinct thing that can go wrong. This is not joint subluxation we’re talking about, not a spinal issue,323 but displacement of the ribs themselves, which can cross over each other like fingers. Laswi et al. found this in 19% of 9th ribs (despite not being true floaters), and 33% of 10th ribs.324 That’s just as startling as the incidence of 10th floaters, and much more direct evidence that associated pathology is probably also more common.
  • Intercostal neuropathy occurs when a rib subluxation pinches an intercostal nerve. This usually feels like a burning pain in a stripe around one side of the torso, but it’s not always that simple. For instance, the twelfth intercostal nerve is linked to the first lumbar nerve … which results in bonus neuropathy: pain in the lower abdomen and groin! And so we get a sub-type of slipped rib syndrome: twelfth rib syndrome.
  • Inflammation around rib tips. A rib tip does not have to pinch a nerve to cause trouble. Any rib with a pointy end — paging Arya Stark — can irritate the tissue around it.
  • Cramping, exhausted, and sensitive muscle is another very likely factor — certainly as a complication, and possibly also a cause or perpetuating factor. The body probably tries to stabilize/immobilize painfully displaced ribs by tightening up the area, which might lead to fatigue and irritation as a consequence. The intercostal muscles in particular can become extremely sensitive. And muscle dysfunction (cramping and/or weakness) might also predispose someone to subluxation in the first place. How many ribs have been pulled out of place?

If there are more floating ribs than we thought, with more free tips… then all of these predicaments are probably also more common and more severe.

“Stick ’em with the pointy end.”

Arya Stark, Game of Thrones (George RR Martin)

Hooked tips on the tenths

Another interesting variation that Laswi et al. found was hooked tips on 10% of 10th ribs — quite common. One in ten people is plenty of people.

The hook is a cartilaginous extension to the 10th rib that curves upwards towards the 9th.325 This is actually nicely depicted in the Gray’s drawing above: that wicked-looking curved extension to the rib!

The hook is clearly just an incomplete cartilaginous connection to the costal margin: rather than merging with the rib or cartilage above, it falls short of it to some degree, making the 10th a floating rib rather than a false rib … but a floating rib with a barb of cartilage pointed directly at the underside of the 9th rib! And free to slip over or under it.

Indeed, over 60% of 10th ribs with hooks were also displaced. Do you know where the tip of your 10th rib is?

Are rib subluxations “a thing”? Skepticism about the skepticism

I have seen several knee-jerk skeptical takes about rib subluxations over the years, overconfidently declaring that they are “not a thing.” Scoffing at the unknown is not exactly skepticism putting its best foot forward. Like so many other things in musculoskeletal medicine, it simply hasn’t been studied much. As Laswi et al. have shown, we didn’t even have the basic anatomy!

The skepticism is mostly aimed at subluxations of the joints between the spine and the ribs, and tends to ignore displacement of rib bodies. I have certainly had my own suspicions about those kinds of rib subluxations, mainly because of their conceptual and anatomical proximity to intervertebral subluxations and some of the worst chiropractic nonsense that has come from that idea. But I carefully, consciously curbed my skepticism until I knew more … and I wish skeptics as a group would do more of that. It might help a bit to undo our strong reputation for throwing babies out with the bathwater.

Granted, there are probably far more more chiropractors opportunistically exaggerating the importance rib subluxation, and profitably overstating their ability to fix it, than there are skeptics cynically underestimating it and rolling their eyes.

But I’m now “fairly” sure that the truth is in the middle: clinically significant rib subluxations of both major kinds probably do exist.326

There’s a short chapter later on exploring self-treatment of slipped rib syndrome.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Part 3

Self-treatment options

How to save yourself from low back pain, or at least avoid getting hurt or ripped off trying

This guide has devoted many pixels to explaining that most back pain feels worse than it is, and to debunking incorrect or over-rated theories of back pain. For the same reason, this part of the book kicks off with a thorough discussion of the special problem of psychological stress, and the surprising potential of informed confidence to resolve low back pain — even some severe, chronic cases. After that, it will continue with a long list of other treatment strategies.

Educated low back pain patients can often manage their own case well enough, with little or no professional help. In all these years of working with patients and clients and readers, I have never yet met a back pain patient who had actually “tried everything,” though many have claimed that. Nearly all had only just begun the journey … and often beginning with the worst options. Once they get more serious and focused about understanding the condition, and working with the best self-treatment strategies, many previously chronic cases improve substantially.

Managing your own back pain has three major components:

  1. The “confidence cure.”
  2. Self-massage and other self-treatment of myofascial trigger points.
  3. Finding good therapeutic help along the way — and a good surgeon if these efforts fail.

Here are the most interesting or important self-treatment options for low back pain, pretty tightly summarized. The rest of the tutorial is devoted to reviewing these and several others in much greater detail. Many are unique to low back pain, and I will review those more thoroughly. Many other treatment or management approaches are applicable to nearly any chronic painful condition or overuse injury, and in those cases I will usually summarize and then link to another article on the site.

  • Given the complex “head games” involved in most chronic low back pain, pursuing greater emotional health and maturity in a general way is a surprisingly worthwhile approach to rehabilitation. There are countless possible approaches to this, but — for readers willing to try something a little odd — I particularly recommend bioenergetic breathing: a form of deep breathing that can provoke a lot of self-reflection and body awareness rapidly.
  • Yoga is an appealing form of semi-recreational exercise for many low back pain patients. At best, you might discover and create the capacity to do things you simply did not know that you could do, either before back pain or since. But yoga has no clearly “active ingredient” for back pain. It might not even be better than doing literally nothing at all, and it is definitely no better than other forms of exercise. The evidence is quite clear: it’s not back pain therapy (or even risk free).
  • Meditation/mindfulness is definitely not everyone’s cup of tea, and it is largely useless as a treatment for any chronic pain… despite it’s absurdly inflated reputation for being evidence-based.
  • Medications are largely futile except for short-term symptom control, and do not work nearly as well as we’d hope. There are some particularly disappointing surprises about medications, like the fact that muscle relaxants don’t actually relax muscle, and marijuana isn’t actually a miracle pain-killer (or even good for anxiety). Voltaren Gel and comfrey cream are noteworthy for being a little more promising and particularly safe. Also, bizarrely, painkillers can actually cause pain — and so getting off certain pain meds may be helpful.
  • Self-massage (for trigger points) is an experimental treatment approach, with only indirect or deeply flawed support from science, but it is well-justified by low cost and risks and high plausibility. Much of the self-treatment section of this book is devoted to several key aspects of it and several tips for working on the low back.
  • Heat and ice are covered in detail below, of course, because nearly everyone tries them. Both are mildly useful, and both can provide good bang for buck, but in general the evidence favours heat. Ice can go badly in some cases, and neither ice nor heat will cure anything. At best, heat might play a non-trivial role in relieving trigger points.
  • Avoiding whatever makes pain worse is a no-brainer rehabilitation tactic. Although it’s obvious in principle, people often neglect a coordinated approach to identifying and eliminating aggravating factors. Insomnia is the most common, pernicious, and unsuspected culprit. Insomniacs should make sleep repair a top priority.
  • Simple rest is the most obvious way of avoiding positions, movements and activities that aggravate the pain. But the evidence is strong that too much rest is just as much a problem as too little. I recommend pursuing a “life in the Goldilocks zone.”
  • Mobilization exercises — thorough, gentle repetitive movements of the back and hips — are a way of systematically exercising in the Goldilocks zone: getting as much stimulation and light exercise as possible without pissing off your back. I provide four specific examples of ideal low back mobilizations.
  • Traction — pulling on the spine in various ways — is something a lot of patients crave. It’s a mixed bag, and it’s too uncertain and risky to pay for it. But if you can keep it cautious and cheap and do it yourself, it’s definitely worth a shot. I will explain how.
  • Stretching is one of the most disappointing of all the classic self-treatment methods. You’ll be hard-pressed to find stories of people who cured their tough chronic back pain with stretching. Stretching is over-rated in general, and seems to be particularly unremarkable for back pain. It’s not very good as a trigger point treatment. Tight hamstrings and other possibly biomechanical relevant muscles can be made more flexible, but don’t seem to have much effect on back pain one way or the other. On the other hand, it can be pleasant and certainly won’t hurt you if you don’t overdo it.
  • Addressing postural stresses is often an easy way to make life a little easier for your back. Decent, comfortable chairs are the major example, of course, but there’s more. Sleeping position is a particularly important one to consider.

So what’s the plan?

If there were proven treatments for back pain, there would be no point in writing this book, now would there? Therefore, debunking is inevitable. The science is firm: all popular treatment options are annoyingly weak.

The best review of back pain science I know is almost entirely bad news: a 2009 paper in Rheumatology (Oxford) by Machado, Kamper, Herbert, Maher, and McCauley.327 It’s a meticulous, sensible, and readable analysis of the very best studies of back pain treatments that have ever been done: the greatest hits of back pain science. (Clinicians with any interest in reading science should definitely read this paper. Many patients will probably find it a little too science-y.)

The results were sad and predictable, a robust evidence of absence:328 “The average effects of treatments … are not much greater than those of placebos.” They were also strong reinforced by another pair of similar studies, which showed not only the same thing, but also that people who participate in all kinds of back pain studies fare no better (or worse) than people who don’t.329330

All that science, and all we know is that nothing popular works well. Of course, this has been obvious for many years to all serious observers of the subject. It’s also obvious to most people with chronic back pain.

What if even these good studies are wrong? What if back pain science is missing perfectly good treatment effects? There are a couple popular theories about this, but Machado et al also did a good job crushing responding to those hopes. So, no, studying just the right patients (subgrouping) probably won’t redeem any treatments.331 And, yes, there is plenty of room for big treatment effects to show up332 — if only they existed.

So give up? Hardly

Given such discouraging science, what are we doing here? Why even bother? Simple: we have lots of latitude for educated creativity and guess work about what might help your low back pain! And almost no latitude at all for a definite treatment plan.

Before I dive into the treatment options, I’d like to respond to a common reader concern that I do too much “debunking,” and not enough telling you what works and exactly how to do it. Although I get few refund requests — way below industry averages — most of the requests I do get are caused by this specific concern: Doesn’t $20 get me a road map to a cure? A step-by-step action plan? The savvy exercise regimen that will make the pain stop?

These things just don’t exist, as I warned in the introduction. I have not sold you a book without mentioning that.

Even if they did exist, prescribing a treatment “plan” is simply out of the question, because every case really is different — that’s not just a platitude. What works for one person really is not going to work for the next. I promise that I’m not holding out on you. I am not a cure salesman, and I will not tell you what you want to hear. There is no specific method or series of logical steps that will reliably cure any kind of chronic pain problem, least of all the tough kind I write books about.

No plan survives contact with the enemy.

Helmuth von Moltke the Elder

Many people reading this probably think plenty of debunking is quite reasonable, normal, and even ethical. But imagine some of the unreasonable expectations I hear from a few customers. For instance, one woman asked me for a refund because my book offered her “only suggestions”! What else is there? What did she expect? Binding arbitration? Click this link for a cure? Free magic wand with every purchase?

Historical perspective and the Age of Hype

The disappointing truth is that there is only a motley assortment of rather underwhelming options with complex pros and cons, but usually more “cons.” Some are better than others, but quite a few are dodgy, for obvious reasons: hope sells, and so there are many more poor options than there should be. Please blame reality for this … not me.

And blame the people who have given you false hope and raised your expectations of musculoskeletal medicine far beyond what it can possibly deliver.

We are living in the “Information Age,” but sometimes it seems more like it’s the Age of Marketing and Hype. An almost unbelievable amount of the information we consume is generated to promote products and services. The result has been an unprecedented flood of being told what we want to hear about absolutely anything.

The reality is that musculoskeletal medicine is surprisingly primitive. Medicine has always had bigger, scarier fish to fry than treating mere aches and pains and injuries, which were barely studied at all until the 1980s. Musculoskeletal medicine is still a cocky teenager, just starting to come of age and figure out that it doesn’t know everything. Even sports medicine specifically, with so much potential funding and relevance to occupational injuries, has been bizarrely slow to build its evidence base.

The trouble with pseudo-quackery: treatments that seem way more legit than they are

The most prominent problem in musculoskeletal medicine today is the prevalence of what I call “pseudo-quackery”: treatments that are about as sketchy as any old-timey snake oil, but seem modern and scientific and mainstream. A few classic examples: laser therapy, ultrasound, platelet-rich plasma, prolotherapy, nerve and muscle stimulation. But there are many more.

These disguised quackeries are actually based mainly on surprisingly stale tradition, speculation, and authority. They generate more false hopes and wasted time, energy, money, and harm than more traditional quackery because they are vastly more popular and very much part of mainstream medicine, or very friendly with it — even many hardened skeptics aren’t expecting snake oil when they go to see a physical therapist or an orthopaedic surgeon.

So musculoskeletal medicine is a minefield, and a lot of debunking just goes with the territory. But it doesn’t mean there’s no good news at all.

The good news

Despite all the debunking and disappointing evidence, I do indeed have positive things to say about several of the options. I have started this part of the book with a summary of all the options, and I will conclude it with another summary of my recommendations, focusing on the positive as much as possible, and what to actually do. Many things are worth trying, even if they aren’t sure things or sitting on any solid science:

We’re told to strengthen this muscle or stretch that one, or inject this substance into an injury, or zap it with heat or electricity or ultrasound … and sometimes it really works, even though placebo-controlled trials fail to validate the treatment. I’m a big advocate of better science to really understand what causes injuries and how to treat them — but in practice, I also believe that sometimes it’s worth trying something, anything, just in case it successfully ‘reboots’ your injury.

Alex Hutchinson, Sweat Science

An encouraging perspective, but of course it doesn’t mean you should try any old nonsense. And you may save some time and money avoiding several others (or at least re-prioritizing them). You may even avoid the heartbreak of those that can do some harm. Knowing what not to do is half the battle, if not more! Understanding the topic well enough to prioritize the imperfect options is actually a huge win, the best you can realistically hope for.

The “negativity” of ratiocination is a surprisingly big topic, often funny, and sometimes profound. I answer the accusation in more detail in a compilation of tales of outrageous hate mail, the ethics and tactics of debunking, what it’s like to (supposedly) be the #1 Public Enemy of Massage (a therapy everyone loves to love), and — my favourite — “advanced negativity,” a discussion of how cynicism is baked into science in the form of the null hypothesis.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Some important things to keep in mind about placebos

Photograph of a plain white bottle with the word “hope” on it, representing false hope and/or placebo.

A placebo is relief from belief: people often feel better simply because they believe they have been treated. More precisely, it is the appearance or illusion of a treatment effect that is not actually attributable to a biological treatment mechanism. It’s a fascinating phenomenon, but its “power” is over-hyped.

This is a standard section in most of my books, covering several key points about placebo that are important context for any thorough discussion of evidence-based treatment options. I do not substantiate any of these points here — all the references are in a more detailed article about placebo.

  • Placebo is not just one phenomenon — “the” placebo effect — but miscellaneous illusions that can collectively create the appearance of an effective treatment. Placebo is complicated!
  • Placebo has a special relationship with pain. Reassurance (placebo) has more potential to relieve pain than most symptoms, because pain is strongly modulated by perception. But that only goes so far.
  • Placebo is not a magical mind-over-pain superpower and its effects tend to be minor and/or brief. It can’t affect injury and organic pathology; it can only tinker with our experience of them.
  • Placebo can also backfire. When a placebo effect wears off — as it usually does — people often fear that they must be really screwed, and then placebo turns to nocebo, placebo’s evil twin: feeling worse because of belief.
  • Placebo potency is driven by whatever impresses the patient with the seriousness and legitimacy of treatment: risks, costs, size, intensity, technology and even odd minutiae like the colour of pills. This is why we have the concept of “therapy theatre” — because so much therapy is all about putting on a show.
  • One of the best ways to impress people is with novel and intense sensations, because the patient can feel the “power” of the treatment. This is the basis of most manual (hands-on) therapies: they are sensation-enhanced placebos (“interactive therapy theatre”).
  • Placebo has been hijacked and re-branded for its public relations value to alternative medicine. If your treatment isn’t evidence-based, no worries: you can still sell the power of placebo! “The power of placebo” is widely, weirdly used as a justification for therapy that can’t beat a placebo.
  • Placebo does not work when you know it’s a placebo, contrary to what many people have heard (based on a couple bad scientific papers). The popular idea of “placebo without deception” is just bullshit, based on an experiment that created a strong expectation effect by inflating the participants’ expectations of placebo. So it was just an odd way of getting to the same phenomenon.
  • Many snake oils supposedly work on animals, and if animals are immune to placebo then the treatment must be legit. But animals (and their biased human observers and caregivers) are definitely not immune to placebo. In fact, with animals there is even more opportunity for an illusion of a treatment effect.

We have a word for medical treatments that only work if you believe that they will, and it rhymes with “gazebo.”

Book Review, Unlearn Your Pain [Schubiner], by Scott Alexander

Is it okay to pay for a placebo?

Many people claim to be happy to pay for a placebo. As long as it works, who cares how? And placebo can work! So why not? This is an extremely common sentiment, raised in most discussions about a treatment that failed to beat a placebo in a fair test (invariably overlooking the fact that neither the treatment nor the placebo actually work very well).

I have no problem with people paying for a placebo as long as their eyes are wide open, but the wider your eyes get the less likely you are to get even a minor benefit.

And paying for things is never completely harmless.

Treatments with unknown efficacy but some plausibility and low risks are the least objectionable placebos to pay for. I’ve tried many such treatments, knowing full well that any effect I enjoy is probably just placebo (or regression to the mean, or natural recovery)… but it might be an actual effect, and I’m willing to pay a little for that chance. I’m gambling on getting a genuine benefit, with a bit of placebo as a consolation prize. So, for me, the plausibility has to be there.

Comic strip of a man standing in front of shelves full of bottles and boxes. On the left, the products are labelled “Placebos.” On the right, they are labelled “Fast-acting, extra-strength placebos.” The caption: “Hmm, better go with these.”

What I want readers to take away from this is that placebo is not therapy. It’s mostly just an over-rated curve ball that accounts for an awful lot of temporary “success” stories.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The confidence cure

In the classic science fiction novel, Dune, the Jedi-like Bene Gesserit use the “litany against fear” to focus their minds and calm themselves:

I must not fear.
Fear is the mind-killer.
Fear is the little-death that brings total obliteration.
I will face my fear.
I will permit it to pass over me and through me.
And when it has gone past I will turn the inner eye to see its path.
Where the fear has gone there will be nothing.
Only I will remain

“The Litany Against Fear” (Dune, by Frank Herbert)

A lot of back pain patients could benefit from the “litany against fear,” because fear is also the back killer. And what’s good for fear? Confidence, powered by knowledge. Educational therapy.

The “confidence cure” is both more than you expect and less than you’d hope. It is surprisingly powerful, but it won’t be enough for everyone. It’s easy in principle, but difficult in practice.

We’ve established that stress is probably a major factor in most low back pain — both life stress and stress about low back pain itself — but reducing stress is a stumper. How do you do that, exactly? Most people really don’t know how to “reduce stress” — the imagination falters after “meditation or yoga” and possibly “beach holiday.” I devote an short chapter below to why you don’t have to bother with yoga and meditation — unless you want to, of course.

So what does stress reduction look like? What actual practical steps can be taken to relieve both the stress of life and the anxiety caused by back pain?

You are already doing the most important thing: reading this tutorial, learning, and starting to master the subject is the number one thing that you can do to “reduce stress” about the low back pain itself, and reduce the impact of other stresses on low back pain. Simply being well-informed is a bit of a magic bullet for back pain.

Knowledge is in every country the surest basis of public happiness.

George Washington, 1790

Some good news? Education probably works, maybe

What I’m calling the “confidence cure” is a more specific version of what’s more broadly known as “pain education,” or more formally known as “cognitive-behavioural therapy,” an umbrella concept for many therapeutic approaches that boil down to the idea that you can break out of back-pain prison by thinking and acting differently. If that sounds a bit weak to you, science agrees: in 2010, a major review of all behavioural therapies had a predictable disappointing conclusion.333

But there are many ways to think and act differently! Until 2013, nothing quite like my “confidence cure” had really been tested properly. I’m pleased to report that it passed its first decent test handily.334 Statistically and clinically significant results for a back pain treatment? Pinch me!335

Specifically, researchers tested classification-based cognitive functional therapy (CB-CFT, or just CFT). You can see why I prefer to call it the “confidence cure.” They describe it as “person centred, body/mind approach to understanding and managing this complex problem” that “targets the beliefs, fears and associated behaviours” of patients. “This approach aims to build self-efficacy, confidence, adaptability and provides hope and opportunity for change for the person with pain.”

That’s the spirit of my confidence cure all right. (There are some hair-splitting differences from other kinds of cognitive-behavioural therapy, but that’s such a boring tangent I’m not even going to put it in a footnote.)

The results of the test were not amazing, but they were satisfyingly good. “Disabling back pain can change for the better with a different narrative and coping strategies.” In particular, CFT beat the benefits of manual therapy and exercise by a wide margin.336 So CFT is now the king of the back pain treatment hill: not necessarily the best back pain treatment, but the best as far as we can tell from the evidence so far.337

By far the biggest problem challenge with the confidence cure is making it practical, understanding and applying it. So, how is it working? What’s the mechanism? Here are three important ways in which educating yourself may directly help low back pain:

  1. Most stress is caused by a lack of information and control. When we understand the nature of our back pain better, it becomes less threatening, and this interrupts one of the most potent parts of the vicious cycle. Stress may continue to be a significant force in other parts of your life — having information about back pain does not give you control over, for instance, your difficult job or your challenging marriage! — but it’s unlikely to continue to express itself as back pain when low back pain no longer seems all that scary.
  2. One of the main reasons that we have back pain when we are stressed is that backs are a good “hiding place” for stress, because most people never suspect that back pain is often an expression of anxiety. It’s a sneaky way to be nervous and frustrated without seeming nervous and frustrated. But once we know what we’re up to, the back is no longer a good hiding place for stress — and the vicious cycle of fear, pain, and immobility is interrupted.
  1. Good information taps into the potency of placebo — the impressive effects of belief and perception on a surprising variety of pain problems. John Sarno claimed that this is the rationale for his successful back treatment method: “ … placebo works at an unconscious level, depending on ‘blind faith.’ What I discovered was that faith or belief in a concept could have a powerful, permanent therapeutic effect if it was based on accurate information” (p89-90), my emphasis. Basically, when you actually understand that your back is not in danger, it feels even better than just taking it on faith.

So that’s how confidence alone might put an end to a lot of back pain. No meditation or yoga required. No heroic efforts — many of which are likely to be ineffective — to eliminate stress in your life (and the effort may be nearly as stressful as the stress you are trying to eliminate). Life is stressful, and there’s not actually all that much that we can do about it. Most people’s lives are full of unpleasant challenges to their peace of mind. Realistically, hardly anyone does enough good yoga/meditation to solve these kinds of problems!

A more practical solution for back pain, therefore, is the knowledge and acceptance derived from education.338 By reading this book, for example. The goal is not to cure all life stress — but to keep it from causing back pain, simply by being well-informed and therefore confident, unafraid, active.

So what does modern pain education look like? It is not reciting pages from a textbook or giving a patient ‘the pain talk’.

Moseley, 2018, British Journal of Sports Medicine

Simple, but not easy

Skeptical? Can treating back pain really be as easy as just learning about back pain? Isn’t this “too good to be true”? it is not actually “easy” at all. Most people require lots of convincing and education before this strategy can have much of an impact — there’s nothing easy about that at all. Most people start out not only not understanding what’s needed for reassurance, but cling to a long list of myths and misconceptions.

If you can actually work through all of that, confidence really does work quite well … and it is a heck of a lot easier than achieving inner peace. This is where the whole notion of “mind-body medicine” really starts to pay off substantively for large numbers of people. The mind has the power to aggravate myofascial pain syndrome and low back pain … but, with some training, your mind also has the power to treat it.339 And this is grounded in good evidence. How people feel about their back pain is an incredibly important factor in the course that it takes.

Jonathan Berkowitz is a statistician at the University of British Columbia whom I am happy to know personally. He is more articulate and more sensible than many members of that profession. He contributed to a large study of factors that predict how quickly people with back pain will return to work; his job was to crunch the numbers. Dr. Berkowitz described to me his amazement as he examined the data. He couldn’t quite believe what he was seeing. No matter how he worked the numbers — and he tried it every which way — the conclusion was unavoidable: the patient’s expectations were the single most significant factor in the duration of their recovery!340 All other factors were significantly less significant.

“Basically, what the numbers clearly showed is that, if you think you’re going to get better and go back to work,” Dr. Berkowitz explained, “you’ll probably get better and go back to work!”

This is why Dr. Bogduk, a very medical man, believes that the most important thing a health care professional can do to prevent acute back pain from becoming chronic back pain is to “treat the patient nice and convince him that there is nothing so horribly wrong” (referenced earlier). Low back muscle pain is all about vicious cycles: the tendency of injury to aggravate MPS, of MPS to aggravate injury, of stress to aggravate MPS, of MPS to cause pain and fear, of pain and fear causing immobility, of immobility causing tissue stagnancy, spasm and MPS, and on and on. Awareness works because it is a way to break the cycle. It gives us some leverage. So, you are likely to feel better if you know that:

  • there is no structural problem
  • you won’t need surgery
  • stress is a root cause, but that doesn’t mean you have to fix your entire life
  • muscles cannot be seriously damaged by MPS
  • you don’t have to give up playing tennis (or squash, or golf, or whatever activity you would hate to lose)

And, therefore, confidence alone can significantly reduce or eliminate a lot of tough cases of back pain — even stubborn ones that have been going on for years. In fact, paradoxically, it may even work particularly well on the toughest cases — tough cases are often tough because of persistent anxiety and fear. Thus, those cases are particularly susceptible to the confidence cure.

It is interesting to speculate how the world got along without back surgery for so long. I suspect that even though the family physicians of seventy-five years ago were unaware of the existence of TMS, they tended not to take back pain or “sciatica” very seriously. Mustard plasters were widely used and probably brought about relaxation of muscle spasm through the heat generated by the plaster. Sometimes folk medicine is more sensible than “modern” medicine. In any case, I suspect that the low-key, non-threatening approach to back problems characterized by an earlier time helped to prevent the kind of long-term, disastrous courses that exist today.

Dr. John Sarno, Mind Over Back Pain, p32

If I were you, I might well be shaking my head by the end of this section. It might seem, yet again, like I am breaking my promise that this is not an “all in your head” theory of low back pain. I still swear that’s not what’s going on here. I’m still talking about how your mind and brain interact with issues in the tissues — genuine provocations, not phantoms.

But you still need more information. In particular, I’ve found over the years — after lots of questions by email — that readers need to understand that the confidence cure is not just a placebo.

If education works, why isn’t it a better known option?

In a 2018 editorial for the British Medical Journal, Dr. Lorimer Moseley explains that “education is universally recommended as first-line treatment for acute and persistent back pain, but it attracts little attention.”341 It’s “always first line but never headline.” Why? It’s not because healthcare professionals “just get it,” unfortunately. Dr. Moseley:

Most doctors (although there are precious exceptions) do not know what [pain education] is, do not know how to do it, do not have the content knowledge and, even if they did, do not have the time. Yet without education, the rest of best practice care— advice to remain active, to exercise or to seek psychological therapy—makes absolutely no sense. To the person with back pain, best practice care without education looks very much like, “we don’t believe you; there is nothing we can do; snap out of it, get back to work and see a shrink.” It is insulting, illegitimatising, and infuriating.

Dr. Moseley wraps up by concisely making the case for back pain education:

Contemporary pain education is potentially more powerful for persistent pain than drugs and as powerful as anything else we can offer. Education is a missing link that would actually make advice to be active, to exercise and to consider psychological therapy a sensible strategy for back pain. Research shows that when someone with persisting pain begins to understand their pain, they actually engage in active, psychologically informed strategies and can have drastic reductions in pain and disability over the next 12 months; for these people, recovery is back on the table.

Truly excellent outcomes are possible for those persistent pain sufferers who take on the journey of retraining their overprotective pain system to be less protective. Contemporary pain science offers compelling reasons to suggest that recovery is within the realms of possible for many persistent pain sufferers. There is genuine hope—not for a quick fix but for a pathway to gradual recovery. It is not for the faint hearted, but people with persistent pain seldom have faint hearts.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

What is the difference between a ‘confidence cure’ and a mere placebo?

The opposite of rational, informed confidence is “wishful thinking” — and it is the single biggest threat to the health care consumer. When you’re in pain, it’s easy to hope for a cure where there are only promises. Snake oil salesmen know this — in fact, they depend on it. They know that people are particularly vulnerable to wishful thinking when they’re suffering from low back pain, and they exploit it in a big way. It’s a particularly scummy flavour of intellectual dishonesty.

The most familiar example of a rational, informed confidence cure: frightened by a strange and unpleasant symptom, you go to a good doctor. She compassionately chuckles and authoritatively explains why you have nothing to worry about: the condition is common and easily treatable. She spells it out for you, and she’s probably right: she is speaking from plenty of direct experience. If she says it’s not a big deal, it probably isn’t! You walk away not only with some “real” medicine … but feeling much better before you even take it.

That’s not the “placebo effect,” even though similar forces are involved. A placebo is based on a deception. But there’s no sugar pill in this scenario.

The confidence cure works its magic on patients in pain in several ways, but the main way is that “pain is an opinion” (Ramachandran) and “100% of the time, pain is a construct of the brain.” (Moseley).342 No matter what is happening in our bodies, it is the brain that decides how dangerous it is and whether or not it hurts and how much. Authoritative reassurance changes the pain equation in a meaningful way. In fact, it literally changes the meaning of your sensations. This is why it is increasingly considered real therapy to educate patients about how pain works:343 to deliberately induce a confidence cure.

All pain problems can freak people out. Many common painful conditions are characterized by strong patient fear and anxiety that — sadly — does not get relieved, because so few health professionals understand pain well enough to offer credible reassurance. For instance, with low back pain, fear is usually way out of proportion to the true severity of the problem. And not only is reassurance tentative and ineffective, many health care professionals will actually scare the patient by reinforcing any number of common, ominous-sounding myths about low back pain.

So reassuring a patient in pain does not create a “placebo,” per se — it’s not fake medicine and a comforting lie. There’s a genuine therapeutic effect there, but it’s based on rational, informed confidence.

But placebo also has a “genuine therapeutic effect,” doesn’t it? Aren’t we always hearing about the “power” of placebo? In both cases, the patient has been led to believe that they are going to be fine, and that belief in turn can actually reduce pain and suffering. So what’s the difference? Why is it fine to aim for a confidence cure … but sugar pills are ethically verboten? It’s complex, but the medical ethicists have been working on that question for a long while now, and there’s a strong consensus: it’s not okay to fool patients. In treating pain, there’s a particularly good reason for that.

A placebo is not a long term solution!

Placebo is not so powerful after all.344 Much of what is labelled “placebo” is actually just the appearance of an effect created by statistical and research glitches. What remains is relatively trivial. Dr. Steven Novella:

Placebo effects are mostly just as much an illusion as precognition or talking with the dead. Pain is the notable exception, which makes physiological sense. Pain is a subjective experience, evolved to have adaptive features that are highly situational. There are times when pain should be very bothersome, and other times when it’s more adaptive to be able to ignore pain. So it is no surprise that mood and expectation have highly influenced the reporting of pain.

To the extent that you can really change people’s moods and expectations about pain, they really will experience less pain and suffering — but often that is still not much better than an illusion. The big problem with a placebo for pain is not just that it’s ethically wrong to get it by lying to patients, even for their own good, but also that it doesn’t work very well. The therapeutic problem with fooling people with a pure placebo is simply that most people rarely stay fooled for long! And when they wise up, they usually end up more hurt and scared than ever before.

And bitter.

But if you can get the same soothing effect on pain without lying, not only are you morally safer, but — and this is the important part — you also get a much more robust effect.

For instance, consider the example of a true snake oil, a therapy that is expensive and totally bogus. (It’s so tempting to give examples! But I’ll steer clear of that, so that the point isn’t derailed by controversy. Or manufactroversy.) Initially, a placebo effect will be powered by mostly the charm and conviction of the seller, and the desperate hopes of the patient. But most patients — even many of the suckers — have that little voice pestering them: “Is this stuff crap? Did I just waste my money?” Rather than true confidence, most people who’ve spent a bunch of money on questionable therapy are watching anxiously for the first sign that they wasted their money.

And of course those signs come quickly, because it’s a bogus therapy. That’s when hope rapidly turns to ashes — so much for that nice placebo effect.

The beauty of an ethical confidence cure is that you get a much more lasting effect — one that is much less likely to be taken away from you later by the discovery that you were being ripped off or just told what you wanted to hear. That’s the difference: a placebo is not a long-term solution, but rational confidence based on good information is. And it’s a huge difference. Therapy must not induce placebo through disingenuous means or use that weak sauce to justify treatments that have no other effect.

Knowledge reduces uncertainty, which reduces stress and anxiety, which reduces pain. It is the job of shamans, not health care professionals, to bullshit their way into a placebo effect for their clients.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Good, better, best: coping, easing, and curing

This chapter is about “coping,” which sounds kind of lame. But here’s the thing: when it comes to back pain (more than any other chronic pain condition I can think of), coping is actually a lot more potent and useful than it sounds.

From a 1998 paper by Hadler and Carey, “Low back pain: an intermittent and remittent predicament of life”:

The tragedy of the past 50 years of the approach to the management of back pain in advanced countries is that we’re treating the wrong illness. We hear the patient complain of back pain when we should be hearing the patient proclaim, ‘My back hurts, but I’m here because I can’t cope with this episode.’ If we could learn to listen to this chief complaint, perhaps we can tackle the impairment in coping, which is the illness that renders the predicament of back pain intolerable.

Is that really what the patient is proclaiming? I have a love/hate relationship with this statement. On the one hand, back pain is largely incurable, and there’s a lot to be said for focussing on the more achievable goals of symptom relief and better coping. “Just give me the good stuff, doc!” Whatever that is. (It’s obviously not opioids anymore.)

But I’ve also seen many people legitimately cured of back pain: diagnosed and treated competently, with a genuinely happy ending. Sometimes without any apparent change in their bodies. It’s not like anything anatomical got “fixed.” I have seen too many stories like that to be comfortable framing the desperate pleas of patients as a request for help merely “coping.” Why not both? I am sure that, in addition to coping, patients also want diagnosis and treatment — which is undoubtedly possible in many cases.

Perhaps the lack of widespread back pain expertise is a more serious problem than failing to “listen to this chief complaint.” We’re not “treating the wrong illness,” we’re treating it ineffectively, because we don’t understand it well enough yet: too many known unknowns and unknown unknowns. Too much of a tangle of factors in many if not most cases.

But then, on the other hand, maybe good coping is actually the road to a cure for some patients.

Wait, what? Did I just say that “coping” could lead to a “cure”? Come again?

The hiearchy of treatment goodness falls apart when applied to back pain

The best possible treatment for any condition is a cure — removing the problem, like pulling the thorn from the lion’s paw. A specific cure is routinely impossible in low back pain. It is only possible in a handful of cases where a treatable cause be definitively diagnosed. But we usually have no idea where the “thorn” is, and there may not be one at all, because it’s so clear that the injury model of back pain doesn’t explain most cases.

The next best thing to a “cure” is effective symptom relief, which doesn’t actually solve anything, but at least you feel better. This is goal of all pain medications. Symptom relief is the only goal in cases that are dominated by chronic pain, since the cause of the pain is usually unknown and/or incurable. Symptom relief often gets a bad rap just because it’s not a cure, but don’t knock a bit of good symptom relief if you can get it!

The next best thing after symptom relief is effective coping strategies: things that in no way improve the condition, but they do make you happier and more functional in spite of it. This gets a bit weird because “happier” and more confident people — as I explained in the last chapter — may actually feel less pain, and so effectively that gets upgraded to true symptom relief.

And then it gets even weirder, because maybe the root cause of the problem is actually sensitization — pain driven by fear, anxiety, stress, catastrophization — and so making that better could even get upgraded to curative.

In other words, what is typically seen as the bottom tier of back pain management, mere coping, might be very important. The entire field of occupational therapy is largely devoted to helping people be happier and more functional in spite of their chronic pain, but the great hope is that doing so will actually relieve symptoms or even solve the problem in some cases.

This is a wide-angle view of the justification for all kinds of squishy approaches to back pain treatment that are not obviously “curative,” but should still be taken seriously.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Stress relief and the tyranny of meditation and yoga

Stock photography of three women doing yoga in a brightly lit gym, with exercise balls in the background. They are all in warrior’s pose.

Relaxing? Or scary?

Yoga classes are allegedly relaxing — in reality, they can be emotional pressure cookers, inflicting intense performance anxiety & self-consciousness about fat & fitness on the participants.

For those who like the idea of meditation and yoga as therapy for any chronic pain, there are other chapters for that:

  • Meditation [work-in-progress, fall 2022]
  • Yoga (new as of Oct 2022, and this chapter is particularly substantial)

But if they are not your cup of tea? This chapter is for you. If this particularly interests you (or you want to share it with someone), there’s also a longer, free standalone article about this, which tackles several more related topics: more detail about risks and harms, the bad attitudes and toxic belief systems of yoga and mediation, alternatives to yoga and meditation, and the “condescension” factor.

Mindfulness and contortions on a thin foam mat are not everyone’s cup of tea. And, although I know this will shock you, these activities are not as scientifically validated as the hype suggests — especially as treatments for back pain and other common musculoskeletal issues, where yoga looms particularly large.

The reputation and popularity of yoga and meditation are oppressively immense, eclipsing other options. People feel that they “should” try them to reduce stress and contribute to a healing process, and have trouble thinking of any other way of responding to pain and stress.345 They actually feel guilty for not trying them or for not liking them. Tyranny!

Yoga and meditation obviously appeal to lot of people for a lot of reasons. Yoga in particular may be good for our general health and fitness.346 But rest assured that they are not the only options — particularly for relief from stress and pain — and no one should be feeling guilty about passing on them. You have my permission to ignore them. And I think some people need that permission.

Comic strip, 4 panels, all featuring two ancient soldiers standing up, and several more lying on the ground in front of them, seemingly dead. Panel one: one live soldier asks, “How do we know if they’re actually dead or just pretending?” Second panel, the other soldier shouts: “I have chronic pain!” Third panel: four of the seemingly dead soldiers speak up, saying “Yoga!” “CBD!” “Lasers!” “Meditation!” In the fourth panel, the two live soldiers proceed to finish off the soldiers who revealed themselves as just pretending.

People just cannot resist suggesting their favourite remedies to people who say they are in pain. Yoga & meditation may actually be the two single most popular choices.

Many people are poor candidates for yoga and meditation

Many productive, energetic people find it difficult — almost alien — to invest in subtle or indirect methods of self-improvement, and find the quiet challenges of meditation particularly exasperating. Many are allergic to the many bogus claims associated with it (like reprogramming your DNA? sheesh).

Mindfulness is particularly a mess: excitement about its potential reaches far beyond the evidence.347 Even for those who can get past all the bullshit and New Age nonsense may still have a personality conflict with the slow pace and calmness required, and their learning curve will feel steep; climbing it while coping with chronic pain, or acutely stressful life events, may not be a practical solution — and yet these are the very conditions when people are most likely to think they should “try yoga.”

But many people never liked the idea to begin with! You know who you are, and you’re not alone. Plenty of you suspect that all “that flaky stuff” is a cure that’s worse than the disease. You are more likely to be successful reducing stress the standard “Western” way, by “blowing off steam” with exercise! Which can be a great idea, except that blowing off steam is often not an option if you are struggling with pain and injury. In fact, one of the biggest stresses in your life may be the loss of exercise, which was the only stress-management strategy you took seriously.

Many people have noticed this exasperating catch-twenty-two: they noticed that stress aggravates their pain … yet they have to solve their body problem before they can exercise again as a way to reduce their stress! Exercise can’t be the solution to stress (or pain) when you’re in too much pain to do it.

That’s frustrating. Stressful, even!

Are yoga and meditation popular? Yes. Mainstream? Not so much

As popular as yoga and meditation are in the world today, they are still not quite mainstream.348 (Indeed, they are part of the “popularity myth” of alternative health care.349)

Even most of my massage therapy clients — several hundred of them from 2000–2010, and a “skewed sample” if there ever was one — had only dabbled in them, at best. (Nor did they ever report any success more significant than “taking the edge off” their stress or their aches and pains.)

When corporations start prescribing yoga and meditation

Y&M may not be truly mainstream, but it’s certainly not for lack of trying! This is a truly perfect example of what I mean be the “tyranny” of yoga, so good that I knew the second I saw it350 that it would have to be prominently added to this article (which hasn’t change much in years):

EVERY COMPANY: We’d like to promote mental health in the workplace.

EMPLOYEES: How about hiring more people so we feel less pressured? And maybe increase our pay a bit so we can keep up with the spiraling cost of living, so we’re not so stressed out?

EVERY COMPANY: No, not like that. Try yoga and meditation.

Hilarious and all-too-true. Don’t get me wrong, yoga can be great, but all normal people know it’s an irritating and inadequate default prescription for stress.

The potential harm of yoga and meditation

This is basically a chapter about why you shouldn’t feel obligated to try yoga and meditation, especially as rehab for your injury, or medicine for your painful problem. How’s “it could make things worse” for a reason?

Yoga in particular isn’t entirely safe, and even meditation has risks! Yes, seriously.

Independently of the direct safety issues, yoga and meditation instructors also aren’t exactly known for their love of science, medical knowledge, or staying in their lane — so you can get some pretty bad advice from them.

But of course the most likely risk is simply wasting your time and money on an activity you don’t actually even like, but you pursue anyway because you think you “should.”

Even weirder hazards of yoga and meditation: you might join a cult (oops)

“Maybe people who meditate for an hour/day are happier because they live a life that affords them an hour/day to meditate.”

Ginny Hogan, Twitter, Dec 17, 2021

Ginny made a good, basic point about self-selection and confounding factors. But the defensive and condescending responses! Yikes! They demonstrate the problem this article is about: too many fans of meditation are far too easily offended, ideologically overzealous, almost cultish. It’s a huge turn-off for most normal people. And it gets worse.

In early 2021, Cécile Guerin reported for Wired that the yoga world is riddled with anti-vaxxers and QAnon believers: “In my day job, I monitor the spread of online disinformation and conspiracy theories. I never expected to find them at my yoga class.”

Seems like one of the most obvious places for them in the world, based on my direct experiences with yoga classes, not to mention all the yoga nuts in my inbox over the years, demonstrating their spiritual sophistication by shrieking at me in all caps — the “Fuck you, Namaste!” phenomenon.

When you open your mind too far, all kinds of crap can get in there, and that’s why New Age bullshit is not harmless. Guerin’s findings are a textbook example of how magical thinking is a slippery slope to much more dangerous and all-consuming beliefs. If a place is badly polluted with pseudoscience, it’s not a “healthy” place — it can’t be. And it’s downright hard to avoid the flaky nonsense in yoga studios.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Another %!@&*!! personal growth opportunity

Conventional self-improvement and life-improvement (problem solving) is the most straightforward alternative to yoga and meditation for general stress relief.

For people struggling with a difficult and slow healing process — low back pain or otherwise — I recommend that they “get personal” with their problem. Sometimes it’s a matter of just learning new coping skills for a pain. And sometimes people need to come to terms with the fact that years of self-sacrificial workaholism, for example, may be the primary factor in severe chronic pain. Usually the truth is in the middle: healing requires a complex mix of coping skills. Through this kind of learning, many people find long term relief.

Whilst the problem is superficially a physical one, the real challenges faced by someone with chronic pain are mental. Mental state is the biggest modulator of physical pain. Things hurt more when you’re stressed or sad, and the increased pain makes you both stressed and sad. The way out of this vicious circle is a wholesale change to how you perceive fear, suffering and setbacks.

How chronic pain has made me happier, by Rob Heaton

Psychotherapy has a role here to: not as therapy for back pain, but with the much broader goal of general sel-improvement Unfortunately, rare indeed is the low back pain patient who will even consider seeking psychological help as a component of treatment, even though it seems to be an obvious opportunity. Even when people do believe that emotional stresses are a major factor in their low back pain, the stigma against “psychological” and stress-related problems is strong! So strong that many people will avoid the most straightforward solutions to it.

But, gosh … ! If you are suffering from severe symptoms that are a physical expression of a life out of balance, it makes sense to see a mental health care professional! Doesn’t it? Food for thought.

Of course, there are nearly infinite ways of approaching this challenge, and I’ve written about it in several ways. I discuss the theory in detail Why Do We Get Sick?, and I get more practical in Pain Relief from Personal Growth.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Yoga has no “active ingredient” for back pain (but it’s still good exercise)

Stock photography of a woman lying on the floor doing yoga, hugging one knee to her chest.

In my first year as a Registered Massage Therapist in Vancouver in 2000, I had not yet turned into a curmudgeonly skeptic, but there were early warning signs. I went to an Iyengar yoga class, billed as therapeutic for patients with pain. A mentor had told me it was an advanced class, ideal for healthcare pros, a kind of continuing education.

I was not impressed. I saw problems. I started nitpicking in my head, and I interrupted the instructor to correct her on a point of anatomy. “Actually…”

Yes, I was that guy: I tried to publicly mansplain anatomy to a yoga instructor the first time I attended her class. But it gets worse! I also got it wrong, despite warning myself to be careful. That memory still burns more than twenty years later.

But here’s the thing: I was right in spirit! Not about that anatomical detail — I surely did screw that up — but I was right about the big picture, right to be underwhelmed and annoyed by the “therapy theatre.” Because most of that “advanced” content was bogus and mostly irrelevant to any kind of pain.

If I’d said, “Actually, yoga has no special active ingredient that is good for back pain,” I would have been right. Obnoxious! But correct.

Does yoga help back pain? I have bad news and less-bad news

Most people assume that yoga is good for back pain, but is that true? Is it “clinically proven”? When tested rigorously, when you filter some of the noise out of the data, do back pain patients get results from yoga?

This question is plagued with a good-news-bad-news answer. Or maybe a bad and less-bad. The answer depends entirely on what you’re comparing yoga to.

If you mean “does it work better than nothing,” well… maybe. According to the science, yoga might be a little bit more helpful than doing no other kind of exercise or therapy, but it’s not nearly as clear as we’d all like it to be. That’s the less-bad news.

But if by “work” you mean “does it work better than other kinds of exercise” — is there something special about it — the answer is a major blow to yoga pride. Yoga doesn’t even get a lousy “maybe” from science on this question. The evidence is blunt: all exercise is equally mediocre as therapy for back pain.

Science isn’t even quite sure that yoga is better than nothing

Many scientific studies declare yoga effective compared to doing nothing,351 but the best ones aren’t so encouraging:

  • A 2017 paper from The Cochrane Collaboration, purveyor of fine meta-analyses (pooling data from many studies), an organization that is notorious for pulling their punches.352 But on yoga for back pain? Cochrane crushed yoga hopes more than usual, concluding that it has no meaningful benefit … compared to nothing. There’s plenty more-study-needed uncertainty too, but the paper is a surprising bummer from this particular source: they acknowledge some short term benefits, but they also make it clear they were trivial.353
  • A 2022 review reported the same small effects without bothering to say they were trivial, but it’s right there in the data. How much did yoga help with pain, temporarily, compared to “nothing”? Less than a point on a ten-scale.354
  • Or there’s the 2020 meta-analysis that concluded that yoga “might” be helpful.355 Hardly a ringing endorsement.

Yoga versus other exercise/therapy

Millions of people assume that yoga is special in some way, better for backs than any other common form of exercise, or at least most of them. Alas, science unanimously declares yoga to be the loser here. No cherry picking required.

  • The 2022 study above was quite positive about yoga-versus-nothing, but strongly negative on yoga-versus-exercise: “Not associated with any significant differences.” Translation: epic fail.
  • A 2021 trial — a pretty good one in the journal Journal of Pain — compared yoga to both physio and eurythmy (a kooky movement therapy and/or performance art). All had equally trivial benefits.356
  • But who needs quality and objectivity? Even the Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine — not exactly known for publishing discouraging words about alternative therapies — concluded that yoga is no better than other exercise.357
  • One study even found that yoga didn’t even help anxiety and depression in back pain patients.358 This undermines the hope that the socialness of yoga is important.
  • A widely cited 2011 trial found that yoga was no better than stretching for back pain … and neither was all that good.359
  • Maybe yoga is better than pumping iron? Not according to the a 2017 trial.360

And so on. There are no important exceptions to this theme in the research.

Buts! The two main objections to the science

Yoga teachers, as a breed, are not known for their love of science, and many will scoff at the studies for two legitimate reasons:

  1. Didn’t study the right kind of yoga. Researchers are not fools and don’t get their yoga teachers from a dollar store, and what they are studying is mostly pretty decent yoga. But of course it is possible that some yoga works better for back pain, and this has been missed.
  2. Didn’t study the right kind of back pain patients. And of course it is also possible — almost certain, in fact — that yoga works better for some kinds of patients than others. In fact, we know it does.361 But the same people responded just as well to physical therapy. In other words, the study didn’t the “right people” for a yoga class, but people with back pain that was just easier to treat by any means.

These concerns about the research have serious theory-versus-practice problems. It’s not clear if the challenge/opportunity of “sub-grouping” is hope or hype. (There’s a scientific paper that addresses that question directly362 — which shares a lead author with one of the better reviews of yoga for back pain.363) It might pay off someday, but it could also be a wild goose chase.364 Back pain is just too hard to diagnose and classify.

For now, no one actually knows what the better yoga really is, or who the right sort of patients actually are. And, even if the smartest of smartypants did somehow know, they would be one voice in the wilderness. Most yoga in the real world would carry on being mostly the wrong yoga for mostly the wrong people, with mostly mediocre effects and just a sprinkling of both success stories and bad outcomes at the edges of the bell curve.

You can keep the hope alive by speculating that yoga is perfect for some back pain. Just don’t kid yourself that you’ll ever really know who.

Speaking of bad outcomes…

Photograph of a woman in “child’s pose” in her living room. Right beside her, a golden lab is in a nearly identical pose, looking bored and sleepy.

Yoga safety: exercise can backfire, and so can yoga

An awful lot happens in yoga classes that seems like it could be a bad idea if you have back pain, and people do get hurt. This is hardly a surprise in an industry so polluted with pseudoscience and myths. Some foolishness is inevitable.

But what cannot help much usually can’t hurt much either, and science confirms it unanimously: yoga is as generally safe as it is ineffective, and serious harm is extremely rare. Big backfires probably takes reckless and incompetent yoga instruction — which, to be fair, is definitely out there, and people need to know that.

But little backfires? Just getting somewhat worse instead of better? That is actually routine.365 In one trial, pain worsened in 12 of 156 people366 — and a rate of 10% means that one or two people in every class are going to get worse, and that is not great. Pissing off some of hurty backs is not what anyone wants.

Harm isn’t a deal-breaker. But you have to get something in exchange for the risk! So these risks, although they aren’t terrible, are hard to justify if yoga doesn’t deliver a clear win to everyone else — which is definitely does not. (Especially if you don’t even like it.)

And now consider the scale of the yoga industry: even if it was only 5% of cases getting worse, that translates into millions of people getting worse instead of better.

That yoga-bashing article in the New York Times

For a while in 2011, the New York Times’ most-shared article was a “yoga bashing” article by William Broad, How Yoga Can Wreck Your Body367 describing potentially serious yoga hazards, such as stroke. Many yoga apologists368 thought Broad’s piece was "sensationalistic." It’s not a perfect article by a long shot,369 but it raised legitimate concerns about a gap between a widely held belief (“most yoga is safe and beneficial”) and the more likely reality: some fairly common postures and practices are a little dangerous, and there not be enough yoga-specific benefits to justify even small risks. He also makes a very good point that many more extreme forms of yoga have truly ridiculous rationales.

All athletic activities have both rewards and risks. The problem here is that yoga’s benefits for back pain patients are uncertain long shots — and people also have many other sketchy ideas about what yoga can supposedly do for them.

The mild but real safety issues are well worth considering. Plus, you could crush your cat. So be careful!

Only one active ingredient of yoga is taken seriously: the core exercise

Yoga contains multitudes, and maybe it’s good exercise because of the prana, or the socializing, or the mindfulness, and people enjoy speculating about what the active ingredients might be. But almost all yoga for back pain is mostly very physical. You don’t see advertisements for Tantric or Jain yoga for back pain. You don’t see a half dozen yoga studios in every city claiming that Hindu spiritual teachings are what makes jivamukti yoga work for back pain.

No, it’s always the hatha yogas, the most secular and fitness-focussed, especially the Iyengar and Vinyasa traditions. Everyone thinks yoga is good for back pain specifically because it’s good for your core strength and coordination, and more generally the “optimization” of posture and spinal alignment.

This is why nearly all yoga studios preach the core exercise gospel, and the question “does yoga work” is basically the same as asking if core training works… and we already know the answer to that. We can infer it from all of the evidence about yoga, but there’s also an avalanche of data about “core” fitness achieved by many other means. See:

The gospel of core exercise is the solution to a largely imaginary problem, fear mongering about a mostly obsolete and simplistic view of back pain that has been debunked by back pain experts and undermined by the science for decades — mostly based on the strong evidence that people routinely hurt without spinal problems, and vice versa.

But the myth marches on, barely dimmed — thanks in large part to yoga!

Yes, some back pain is structural in character, and we shouldn’t throw those babies out with the bathwater. But the last thing most of those cases need is yoga. (And, even if spines needed changing, or more “support,” yoga does not do it.370) Meanwhile, there are many other more common and important drivers of back pain: messy biochemical and sensory factors instead of biomechanical. The most important thing to understand about back pain is that it rarely has one clear mechanical cause, and even more rarely one that can be helped by yoga — if ever.

Can yoga-for-back-pain classes still help people? Yes… but they can also hurt!

Yoga truly is a nice way to exercise, and that can help many people. It’s not gargling bleach or psychic surgery. And that is why I haven't relegated this chapter to the bogus cures section. It was a close call, but there's enough value in exercise we enjoy — regardless of any specific power to affect back pain — that I do think that people who like yoga should do yoga when they have back pain.

But they should do yoga with back pain, not for back pain. And cautiously. And ease up at the first sign of worsening back pain.

I do not think that teaching yoga as therapy for back pain is a good idea. Not enough people are helped, and too many get harmed, both directly and indirectly.

Are yoga teachers qualified to treat low back pain?

You can’t diagnose spinal tumours or infections from the front of a yoga class, which is basically why yoga instructors are only barely qualified to teach yoga to back pain patients, but never for back pain. There are many causes of back pain, and some of them are scary, so the minimum qualification to help people with back pain is being well educated about red flags (and also having the time for discussion and examination).

Is it possible to teach an evidence based “yoga for low back pain” class? Are you teachers qualified to treat back pain?

Yes. But it would be a strange and ironic class! Done right, it would undermine its own purpose, while providing what people actually need: some relief from the false hopes, red herrings, and nocebos provided by so many others. There are many back pain myths, yoga’s value for back pain is one of them, and debunking is effective.371 And why not have a class so humble that it’s almost laughing at itself? Seems rather Buddhist, actually. The right kind of students will find all this very refreshing… and yoga is still going to happen, and that’s still a good thing even if it’s not a “special for back pain” thing.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Beyond the confidence cure: what else can you do for low back pain?

I’ve now done all I can do to convince you that it’s all about the trigger points, and that the trigger points in the low back are particularly vulnerable to anxiety and stress and fear, and your first priority is to stop worrying about your back, and then perhaps address some of the broader emotional challenges in your life.

However, many of you will be relieved to know that, yes, there are other more tangible treatment options!

I have repeatedly promised that this tutorial does not subscribe to an “all in the head” explanation for back pain! And it does not. Trigger points are a real physical problem. As important as it is to have confidence and not be frightened and intimidated by your back pain, there are also many things you can and should do to help relieve trigger points and promote recovery.

And, in any case, no matter how true it might be that psychological factors are relevant, everyone needs something to do when they are treating any chronic pain problem. You need tasks, something to work at, because action is one of the best cures for anxiety. So the next several sections are devoted to many more specific and practical therapeutic recommendations. Most of them, one way or another, are about treating trigger points.

But please bear in mind that all of these other self-treatment methods are probably ultimately less important than knowledge, awareness, and acceptance!

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Centralization, directional preference, and (mostly) extension exercises

Funny thing about back pain: some patients, especially the ones that also have leg pain, will feel quite a bit better when they move or position themselves in specific ways. For example, you might notice that leaning backwards repeatedly eases your leg pain — either it stops shooting quite so far down the leg, or it even disappears for a while. In some cases, pain right in the back itself might back off.

This well-documented phenomenon is cryptically known as centralization — because the pain retreats from the periphery into the center of the body. It’s an odd term. A better term would probably be “focalization” (hat tip to David Poulter), but “centralization” is extremely well established.372

A minor visual pun, graphics of a road sign that reads “good sign.”

“Centralization” is a good sign (yuk yuk).

The centralization thing happily happens in roughly 40% of people with back pain — happy because it is strongly linked to better outcomes.373 You want to be a centralizer! If you experience centralization, that is an official Good Sign..

This-a-way feels better: the “directional preference”

Whatever repeated or sustained movement or posture triggers centralization is your directional preference. In other words, if your pain “centralizes” when you bend backwards repeatedly, then your “directional preference” is extension. You prefer moving in that direction.

Strangely cryptic terms for something that really boils down to feeling better when you move one way rather than another.

This simple concept has become a staple of physical therapy for back pain. It comes from the McKenzie Method of “Mechanical Diagnosis and Therapy” (MDT), created by New Zealand physical therapist Robin McKenzie (1931–2013) — one of the best known “systems” in the body-fixing business. I’ll address MDT more generally in another chapter. This chapter zooms in on the one thing that MDT is almost defined by: extension exercises.

Or flexion for a few people.

And maybe side-bending for a handful.

Dr. Stephen May is a researcher strongly associated with MDT (he literally “wrote the book” on it, with McKenzie). In an interview for PT Pro Talk (episode 95), he said:

“I don’t think there’s anything like it in terms of its high prevalance rate, its potential to indicate a good prognosis, and its potential to indicate a management strategy. And if a clinician doesn’t take such a gift into account they would seem to be shooting themselves in the foot in a very daft way.”

Later in the book I will dig into this a bit more and push-back against that cockiness in a more general chapter about McKenzie/MDT. For now, I think there’s enough truth in Dr. May’s opinion that some DIY experimentation is fully justified: it’s evidence-based enough, easy enough, cheap enough, and safe enough that it should be in everyone’s bag of tricks.

Photo of a woman stretching in yoga cobra pose, at home or in yoga studio with big window and bright light, a lush house plant, and a tree outside.

Yoga’s “cobra” pose is an excellent example of a simple back extension exercise — which might be your (directional) preference. Extension is the direction most back pain patients prefer, if they have any preference.

Simple self-help for back pain based on centralization and directional preference

Self-help, really? Based on this rather technical-sounding phenomenon? Well, McKenzie himself published self-help books about it: Treat Your Own Back. So I think it’s fair game.

A 2004 trial asked “does it matter which exercise?” Meaning, does it matter if you exercise in the direction of your directional preference, versus the other way? 72% of the people the researchers worked with had a directional preference, and those folks did better harmonizing with their preference than clashing with it… regardless of what that preference was. That is, if they preferred extension — as most do — then extension exercises produced much better results than flexion. But it was vice versa if they preferred flexion.374

Simple illustration of a woman swinging her hips in a circle.

There are three directions to prefer: back (extension), forward (flexion), or the side as illustrated here. Sidebending is the rarest directional preference & this image emphasizes its simplicity. Just… bend to the side!

Nifty little study. Unfortunately, it is also an isolated bright spot in a generally disappointing literature on the McKenzie method. But it’s something.

Patients can and should exploit this concept by testing themselves for centralization and a directional preference, and then spending time in moving and resting in that preferred direction. Obviously not everyone will succeed at this, but quite a few will, and it’s quite safe to experiment with. In more detail…

There is no step three

Step One: Test for centralization by cautiously experimenting with a variety of spinal movements and positions, looking for clear signs of your symptoms contracting into the center of the problem — less pain sprawl, and relief that persists for at least a little while. There is no need to do anything intensely, or to repeat anything that obviously makes you feel worse.

In the best-case scenario, this is easy and obvious and you can confirm it in minutes. In the worst-case scenario, it might take several sessions of experimentation over a few days to pick up on the signal. If there’s no sign of centralization after a few days, it’s time to give up: you are not a centralizer! (Not yet, anyway. It can change! You can re-test every week or two.)

Only centralizers should proceed to step two.

Step Two: Once you have determined that you have a clear directional preference… use it! Spend time every day moving your spine that-a-way. Or resting with your spine curved in that direction. If it hurts or makes things worse, just stop — the whole idea here is that you’re emphasizing something that makes your back feel *better*. Not worse.

That’s the basic self-serve version of exercise therapy for back pain based on centralization and a directional preference.

Of course you can also recruit professional help with this: a professional with MDT training and experience (and there are a lot of them around the world) obviously might be able to suggest many refinements, and that may be worth pursuing. But there’s also nothing wrong with experimenting yourself, and that’s going to be enough for many people.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The cannabinoids: marijuana and hemp, THC and CBD — “it’s complicated!”

Photo of marijuana plant.

Perhaps the most interesting & controversial plant in the world.

Cannabis is a plant, most notably marijuana (bred for its narcotic effects) and the major strain of hemp (bred for other purposes). It’s one of the most interesting plants in the world because it produces chemicals with interesting effects, the cannabinoids. The most interesting and famous of those are THC (tetrahydrocannabinol) and CBD (cannabidiol). All cannabis contains THC, CBD, and hundreds of other related compounds, but there’s a lot more THC in marijuana plants, and a lot more CBD in hemp.

THC gets you high (psychoactive effects), and CBD does not. Both are alleged to be pain killers: it’s their most popular medicinal use (either that or as a sleep aid), but CBD is much less studied.

The evidence for pain-killing

So, are cannabinoids effective pain killers? “It’s complicated”!

As a science journalist, I am honour bound to emphasize that cannabinoids are not proven pain-killers. “Proof” is a high bar that has not yet been cleared. A huge 2017 review of the scientific literature on cannabis concluded that there is “substantial evidence that cannabis is an effective treatment for chronic pain in adults.”375 But the review also explains that the evidence shows only modest benefits so far, there is uncertainty about every detail, and significant practical problems abound for both researchers and consumers.

And that was hardly the last or only word. Other reviews of largely the same evidence have been much less optimistic. In 2017, Nugent et al looked at 27 scientific trials of cannabis for chronic pain trials, and it was disappointing: weakly positive for neuropathic pain, and just inconclusive otherwise.376 In 2019, Häuser et al wrote “Cannabis medicines can be regarded to be third-line therapy for chronic neuropathic pain. There are signals of a lack of efficacy for all other chronic pain syndromes.”377

So that’s not great.

THC might have some bonus effects for some low back pain patients because it can function as a muscle relaxant. More about that in the muscle relaxant chapter. But it is one way that THC might be relieving pain indirectly.

A noteworthy new trial of CBD for acute back pain

Studies of pure CBD for pain remain rare, which makes a good 2021 Australian trial noteworthy.378 Researchers tested CBD in the same situation where doctors might normally prescribe powerful anti-inflammatories or opioids: in people who have gone to the emergency room with severe back pain. A hundred patients were given an ibuprofen and paracetamol plus either 400mg of CBD or a bogus pill that looked just like it.

This a fair test in several ways. If CBD actually has anti-inflammatory properties, the people who got a CBD booster should certainly have gotten some extra relief. Severe acute back pain is a tough pain-killing challenge, but we should expect anything touted as “good for pain” to help out at least a little in this situation.

Unfortunately, CBD made no difference at all: it was like it wasn’t even there. •sad trombone• The groups were identical on all outcomes: pain levels, how long the patient stayed, the need for “rescue analgesia” (the Oxycodone), and adverse events.

It is conceivable that CBD alone, or repeated doses, would have performed at least as well as standard meds, and maybe more safely — safer than NSAIDs anyway379 — but it’s a long shot. This was quite a fair test, and CBD just bombed.

Usage guidelines for beginners

If you’re new to marijuana, there’s a bit of a learning curve. Here are some tips:

  • Pure topical CBD creams and oils are overall the safest and most convenient, so they should probably be your first priority to try. THC may be where it’s at, but it’s harder to use…
  • Use caution with THC edibles! Dosing and duration of effect are huge wildcards. You can get way, way too high for comfort — not very dangerous, but scary.
  • Avoid vaporizers that use oil infused with cannabinoids, due to scandalous, tragic safety issues because asshole manufacturers have added other dangerous, un-tested substances380. Dozens of people died in 2019. Died!
  • Infused oils aside, vaping raw cannabis in moderation is quite safe.381 Just take it easy for at least your first three times — just one or two modest inhalations of vapour is just fine to start.

For more detailed information, see Marijuana for Pain.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Over-the-counter pain medications — not miraculous, not worthless

The commonly used over-the-counter pain-killers are Tylenol & anti-inflammatories like ibuprofen.

Nothing you can swallow is likely to significantly relieve back pain. There are so many possible causes and mechanisms involved in back pain that, in principle, one type of relief is probably not going to work well for a lot of people. But that complexity means that it’s also worth experimenting — you never know what might work for any given patient.

And even a little benefit is better than nothing. So it’s worth experimenting — as long as you know how to do it safely. There is nothing wrong with cautiously experimenting with over-the-counter pain medications to occasionally, maybe help tame the cycle of pain, fear, and immobility, as long as you know this is what you are up to. A temporary crutch.

If you do discover a specific pain-killer works even a little better than the others, that can be quite valuable over the years. It’s worth paying close attention to using each major type of pain-killer on at least three occasions. And that could take a while! I suggest you take notes — seriously.382

Prescription medications like opioids, anticonvulsants, and sedatives are discussed below in another chapter.

Regular pain-killers, mainly ibuprofen and acetaminophen

Basically, the regular pain-killers mostly don’t work all that well — which surprises no one.

But they can take the edge off. And that is not nothing. There are days when that is what you need.

Your typical sugar pill placebo is going to net you a drop in pain of 10%, which is also about where “minimum clinical significance” is usually defined. That is, less than a 10% reduction in pain just doesn’t matter — it’s too small. Guess how much pain relief people get from over-the-counter pain killers on average? Just that much. In a 2017 review of 35 studies, both placebos and common anti-inflammatories for back pain had a “smallest worthwhile effect” of about 10%.383 The authors think that “there are no simple analgesics that provide clinically important effects for spinal pain over placebo.”

It’s possible that they just don’t work at all, complete duds, and the only reason they have any effect is placebo — same as the sugar pill. Or maybe their effects are just weak and unpredictable. Everyone’s different, every case is different. These meds probably do more for some cases than others. Our reactions to drugs are surprisingly variable, from person to person, and day to day. This has been well-documented for treating tension headaches with these meds.384

Most drugs work on only about a third of the population, they do no damage to another third, and the final third can have negative consequences.

Craig Venter, extremely famous and spooky smart geneticist (public lecture, Vancouver, May 3, 2011)

To the extent that the anti-inflammatories provide any detectable symptom relief, it may suggest the presence of injury (as opposed to cramp or trigger points). But there is rarely a significant inflammatory process involved in low back pain, or at least not one readily controlled by these medications.

All the over-the-counter (OTC) pain medications are fairly safe in moderation and work in different ways. There are four kinds: acetaminophen/paracetamol (Tylenol, Panadol), plus three non-steroidal anti-inflammatories (NSAIDs): aspirin (Bayer, Bufferin), ibuprofen (Advil, Motrin), and naproxen (Aleve, Naprosyn).

Don’t take any of them chronically — risks go up over time, and they can even backfire and cause pain (rebound headaches).385 Acetaminophen is one of the safest of all drugs at recommended dosages, but overdose can badly hurt livers and it’s the least likely to help back pain (at all?). The NSAIDs reduce inflammation as well as pain and fever, but at any dose they can cause heart attacks and strokes and they are “gut burners” — they can badly irritate the GI tract (even taken with food, and especially with booze). Aspirin is usually best for joint and muscle pain, but it’s the most gut-burning of them all.

Naproxen might be the best of the over-the-counter drugs for most back pain (without significant sciatica, which tends to be tougher to tame).386 The evidence isn’t exactly conclusive, but it’s enough to justify prioritizing naproxen in your testing.

The standard of care has definitely moved on from routine prescription of medication, but “many patients with persisting symptoms also continue to take medicines long-term despite the low likelihood of ongoing benefits.”387 Don’t be one of those people. I think the main point to take away about the medications is that, even if it’s okay to do some cautious experimenting, there’s definitely no point (and plenty of risk) in continuing to take them if they aren’t obviously helping.

Topical anti-inflammatory (diclofenac/Voltaren)

Voltaren gel is an anti-inflammatory medication in a gel that you rub on, and it has a little more potential than the same thing in a pill — but Voltaren is a lot safer.388 Scientists recently found evidence (in rats!389) that it might actually be able to treat pain coming from deep inside the spine — right in the centre — which means it could be a “convenient and safe clinical intervention” for a few types of back pain.390 So convenient and safe, in fact, that it lands in the clinical “why not?” sweet spot. An anti-inflammatory gel will probably fail with many kinds of back pain (the ones that involve no inflammation) … but, unlike with the pills, there’s also virtually no down-side to trying.391 Give it a try.

Voltaren Gel — not exactly a magic bullet, but probably safe, reasonable & worth a shot.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Muscle relaxants (Robaxin, Robaxacet, etc), psychoactives, and sedatives

“Muscle relaxant” is an odd category of drug. There are several drugs that are relaxing, but are not exactly “muscle relaxants” because they are not specifically interfering with the biology of muscle contraction. A true muscle relaxant is essentially a poison that messes directly with muscle physiology.

You really don’t want too much of a true muscle relaxant. It can cross into paralysis. Amazonians used a muscle relaxant … on their poison arrows. Curare poison relaxes you to death. European explorers encountered the stuff early in their visits to North America, and it led to some of the earliest scientific studies in pharmacology.

And yet, on the other hand, it’s not clear that the muscle relaxant drugs are actually interfering with muscle contraction! So it’s a tricky topic.

Muscle relaxants and back pain

Earlier in the book, I discussed the idea of “back spasms.” Refresher: it’s a hopelessly vague description of a sensation that probably doesn’t have much to do with actual cramping of back muscles. Or it’s something more specific, like trigger points — micro-cramps that do involve unhealthy contraction, but aren’t really anything like the popular mental picture of a whole-muscle clenching painfully.

If some kind of back spasm is not a cause of back pain, surely muscle relaxants are largely pointless. Some experts are very cynical about this, describing muscle spasm as a simplistic non-diagnosis with strong emotional appeal to both doctors and patients, and therefore cynically exploited by pharmaceutical companies to sell a treatment (muscle relaxants).392 Some muscle relaxants certainly are marketed specifically as remedies for “muscle pain.” For instance, King Pharmaceuticals claims that Skelaxin produces “fast relief for muscle spasms and back pain.” There are definitely plenty of pros who think that’s just a scam.

Maybe. I’m sympathetic to that point of view, but I’m also not so sure it’s settled.

If muscle spasm is a major factor in back pain, or even a significant complication, then muscle relaxants are obviously useful in principle — and in fact we do know that muscle relaxants are somewhat helpful for acute low back pain (evidence discussion below). But there are also reasons other than actually relaxing muscles why those drugs might work a bit.

And then of course there’s the unavoidable truth that there are many causes of back pain that really have nothing to do with tight muscles. Even an extremely potent muscle relaxant is not going to make any difference if tense muscles aren’t the problem in the first place.

But, if they help you, it’s actually diagnostic. Effective treatment isolates uncomfortable muscular contraction (of some kind) as a cause or clinically significant complication.

Muscle relaxant primer

Muscle relaxants come in many related varieties,393 but only one that is widely available without a prescription: methocarbamol, as found in Robaxin, Robaxacet, and similar brand names.

There are also several prescription muscle relaxants, obscure to most patients, but most notably carisoprodol (Soma), cyclobenzaprine (Flexeril), metaxalone (Skelaxin).

All muscle relaxants are tame cousins of the truly potent sedatives (also discussed below), and can cause significant drowsiness, dizziness, and a laundry list of other common side effects, but there’s also a surprisingly wide range of safe dosage (hard to overdose).

Methocarbamol and friends are not widely used because they are not super effective. It’s probably because they’ve been around forever, because the drowsiness they cause makes them feel more potent than they actually are, and because relaxing muscles just seems like such a good idea to literally everyone, both patients and pros. I think perhaps muscle relaxants have been grandfathered into modern, scientific medicine. Tradition-based medicine, rather than evidence-based medicine.

Muscle relaxants are surprisingly unstudied, like many other popular drugs.394 In particular, good luck finding any study of the effect of these drugs on muscle function. It’s not clear if muscle relaxants actually relax muscles, or if they just make us feel more relaxed.

What evidence we do have is not exactly high quality.395396397

An expert of my acquaintance thinks they are useless specifically at low dosages.398

Acute back pain is the only condition for which there is adequate data. Some muscle relaxants (including methocarbamol) do appear to be roughly as effective for acute back pain as common over-the-counter pain killers399400 — so they can help, but not all that much, and with great potential for side effects. It’s also damning that there doesn’t seem to be much difference between muscle relaxants: “Comparison studies have not shown one skeletal muscle relaxant to be superior to another.”401 So we have a class of drugs that shows little sign of effect, no matter which flavour you use. Whoop-de-do!

Even a prescription muscle relaxant like carisoprodol (Soma) is so impotent that patients will (this is bizarre) actually tense up if they are lied to and told that the drug is a stimulant.402 (The study was quite interesting — if you only read one footnote about a study in this book, this would be a good one to choose.) Clearly the brain is the boss of your muscle tone, and the drugs only nudge us towards relaxation. Bear this fun fact in mind for the discussion of alcohol and other psychoactive drugs coming up — it’s a ray of hope.

And here’s another fun fact: even anasthesia doesn’t truly “relax” muscle.403 It stops voluntary contraction, but it doesn’t eliminate muscle tone. Only death does that, and even in that extreme case the tenacity of the contractile proteins is demonstrated in the phenomenon of rigor mortis.404

Muscle relaxants clearly work at least a little for some people, some of the time, probably usually at higher doses. And they are relatively safe to experiment with, even at higher dosages. In fact, it’s so hard to overdose on them that I even feel comfortable endorsing cautious testing of a larger dosage than what’s recommended on the box. Just don’t go driving, don’t combine with alcohol, and be alert for significant side effects — they aren’t effective enough to bother with if they harass you with side effects.

Narcotic “muscle relaxants” (sedatives like Valium)

If our goal is to loosen up tense muscles that might be causing low back pain, is there any drug at all that will definitely do the job? Any drug that’s reasonably accessible?

Narcotic sedatives, mainly the benzodiazepenes, relax everything. Like the opioids, the benzos are another “nuclear option” — they do interfere with muscle contraction, while also interfering with everything else: like consciousness! Many drugs have highly unpredictable effects, but the benzos are as potent and predictable as cobra venom.

The most famous of all the sedatives is diazepam, AKA Valium, a benzodiazepene. But it is only the most infamous member of a family of rogues, like Klonopin, Ativan, and Xanax.

Just because they are potent — and they certainly are that — does not mean they actually work, or that they work by relaxing muscles. A 2017 study showed that “Diazepam Is No Better Than Placebo When Added to Naproxen for Acute Low Back Pain” (they put the result right into the title).405 However, I suspect that the your-mileage-may-vary factor is huge with these drugs.

As with the milder muscle relaxants, it’s surprisingly unclear whether or not these drugs actually reduce muscle tone, or whether they achieve relaxation indirectly via their potent sedative and psychoactive effects. Those psychoactive effects are a huge wildcard that could account for a wide range of responses. They are primarily used to relieve anxiety and improve sleep, both of which could easily relieve pain on their own.

I don’t think there’s any question that benzos might be a useful tool for treating headache. It seems worth at least considering this treatment option, despite the hazards… which are a big deal.

Benzos really do involve dire risks of physical dependence and addiction, and withdrawal can be nightmarish, dangerous, and even lethal at the extremes. Although it is possible to take and stop taking benzos safely, many people do not get the information and help they need for that. Sadly, I have extensive personal experience with benzo withdrawal, and I’ve written about that in detail.406

However, benzos can be safe if used in moderation for short periods only. They are a dangerous tool, like a gun, which must be respected and used with great caution. If you’re interested in dancing with this devil, don’t just ask your doctor for a prescription: make a point of showing your prudence by asking for only a 2-week supply of a small dosage only (the exact amount varies with the specific drug).407

The curious case of alcohol as a pseudo-muscle-relaxant

Alcohol is hard on your system in many ways. It’s a myth that a drink every night is a tonic, and there really is no safe intake level408 — it is a poison, and it can make people more vulnerable to chronic pain. And yet…

There’s plenty to be said for a glass of wine or beer as a kind of medicine.409 Anecdotally, moderate usage seems useful for taking the edge off nearly any kind of pain. This may be because it’s functioning as a kind of muscle relaxant, or at least as a general sedative.

Photo of a woman in fitness clothes, sitting on a yoga mat, pouring herself a glass of wine. The caption reads: “To relieve stress I do yoga. Just kidding, I drink wine in my yoga pants.

Let me be clear: alcohol is not a muscle relaxant per se,410 not in a biochemical sense (and warning: it also combines dangerously with actual muscle relaxants and sedatives). In a world full of alcoholism and drug addiction, obviously a “prescription” of alcohol has to be offered and taken rather cautiously. But it is highly accessible, cheap, relatively harmless in moderation, and it’s a psychoactive drug — and anything that bends your mind has the potential to be a mild, obliquely effective muscle relaxant.

Combined with the confidence cure, alcohol might be effective, after a fashion. It’s one thing to be anxious about your low back pain and to drink to take the edge off your nerves — only to get right back to it as the effect of the beer therapy wears off. It’s quite another to know that your spine is safe but grouchy, and to drink judiciously to help break the vicious cycle of tension and anxiety itself, instead of just temporarily escaping it.

All psychoactive drugs — anti-depressants, alcohol, marijuana, amphetamines, opioids, benzodiazepines — often seem to help almost any problem, but the emphasis is on seem, because they mainly affect mood: “It is my impression that ‘pain-killing’ drugs improve the patient’s mood rather than take away the pain.” (Sarno)

Mood isn’t nothing. If you are genuinely happy and relaxed… if that’s something alcohol can do for you…

THC as a pseudo-muscle-relaxant

Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) is the most famous cannabinoid produced by cannabis (marijuana). It is another relatively safe and accessible psychoactive drug, probably the only other one that seems tame enough to take seriously for this purpose. (Sure, you could blow your mind with acid or ecstasy and hope for a muscle relaxant effect, but that seems like overkill.) Obviously its accessibility varies widely from place to place as the entire world grapples slowly and awkwardly with legalization.

Like alcohol, THC doesn’t zap muscle tone directly, but it probably can do it indirectly and erratically.

If you are happy and relaxed while stoned, it’s certainly possible that a tense back will get … less tense! Whether it can meaningfully affect the kind of fierce contractions (or trigger points) that can make you miserable in the first place is anybody’s guess, but it seems worth trying.

For this purpose, virtually any strain of marijuana will do, but ideally a THC-rich one (not CBD) because you want the psychoactive effects. Whee!

With pot, there is also the added potential for direct pain relief (discussed above, along with usage guidelines).

And so I do tentatively recommend THC as another “muscle relaxant” worth experimenting with.

Botox?

Botox is the infamous face-paralyzing drug of the stars! It’s a special case, quite different from the other muscle relaxants. Like curare on poison arrows, Botox is outrageously toxic and doesn’t “relax” muscles but just outright paralyzes them, even in small doses. It’s obviously related to muscle relaxants, but it’s a separate topic.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Comfrey makes backs comfy, study claims

A comfrey plant. I harvested a large pile of the stuff once, working for room & board on an organic farm in my wild & free hippie youth.

Here’s a well-I’ll-be-darned study showing the potential for good low back pain relief from a surprising source. These kinds of results are usually published in journals like Herbal Believer and The Journal of Cheering for Herbal Remedies No Matter What. But this is from the British Journal of Sports Medicine, a respectable publication.

Researchers not only found that ointment made from the root of the comfrey plant is an effective treatment for low back pain, but a “potent” one!411 Assuming the experimental results are sound and can be repeated by other researchers, this one’s a rare, clear win for a traditional herbal remedy.

“The results of this clinical trial were clear-cut and consistent,” the authors report. “Comfrey root extract showed a remarkably potent and clinically relevant effect in reducing acute back pain.”

At first glance this certainly looks like an adequate test of comfrey. I will be watching for confirmation studies. Meanwhile, my recommendation to patients is to give it a try: minimal risks and positive results in a credible journal put it firmly in the “worth a shot” category.

I wonder if comfrey has something in common with Voltaren gel (discussed above)? Both are seemingly pretty mild ointments, both with some evidence of effect on a stubborn pain problem. There may well be some overlap in how they work.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

A tale of two tutorials

PainSci also has a very detailed guide to trigger points.

The two guides have a close relationship. The trigger points tutorial was last mentioned several sections back when I was introducing you to the role that trigger points play in back pain. I’m bringing it up again because we’re now going to be talking about treatment options for low back pain — most of which are directly concerned with trigger points, and are quite similar in both tutorials.

This duplication is unavoidable: the subjects are woven together like the snakes of the caduceus. And this is why I sell the two tutorials as a pair. I have made every effort to reduce the duplication, and to discuss trigger point therapy concepts here as they relate specifically to low back pain. For instance, rather than discussing only the general principles of mobilization exercises (as in the trigger point tutorial), I provide some specific examples of mobilization exercises for the low back in particular.

However, it is impossible to eliminate the duplication altogether, and sometimes it is nearly word-for-word for several paragraphs at a time. For instance, a section of the trigger points tutorial about a self-massage technique I call “the bath trick” I reproduced here in full because it is ideal for low back pain. I want readers of both tutorials to know about the bath trick.

However, for detailed trigger point therapy information, please refer to that book-length tutorial, which is about three times the size of this one. The next several sections of this tutorial are an executive summary of the same sections in the book-length trigger point tutorial. It may seem like there’s a lot here, but, trust me, there’s a lot more in the trigger points tutorial!

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Introduction to treating your own low back trigger points

Massage might help back pain. No one really knows, because, surprise surprise, even garden variety massage for back pain hasn’t been studied properly yet, let alone trigger point therapy. It is supported only by indirect and flawed scientific evidence (thoroughly reviewed below). For now, the bottom line is that it’s an experimental treatment, but for many reasons it makes sense to try it — especially self-massage, because it’s free and safe.

In my opinion, most trigger point pain can probably be relieved with a surprisingly small amount of simple self-massage, or with a helping hand from the right therapist in those hard-to-reach spots! Although trigger points can be weird and stubborn at their worst, the majority of them are usually easy to get rid of with a just little rubbing.

Dr. Janet Travell wrote that “almost any [physical] intervention” can relieve a trigger point. And self-massage is usually the simplest, cheapest, and most effective intervention. How can so little be so effective? How can such a minor treatment work?

The vicious cycle taking place inside a lesser trigger point is not difficult to disrupt. The knot may not be all that tightly contracted in the first place. The accumulation of metabolic wastes is relatively small compared to a severe trigger point, and it’s fairly easy to squish out with gentle pressure. The neurological dysfunction is not usually particularly entrenched, and it’s fairly likely to change in response to minor stimuli. Adhesions (“scarring”) are not a factor in trigger points that haven’t been around for long. And isolated trigger points are much easier to manage than the large groups of trigger points that are typical of full-blown myofascial pain syndrome.

The pillars of self-treatment for low back pain trigger points are:

  1. Self-massage — the creative, persistent stimulation of musculature in the low back with pressure, applied a little bit with thumbs and fingers and fists, but mostly with massage tools like tennis balls or a massage stick.
  2. Mobilization, or “massage with movement” — gentle but precise rhythmic movements that stimulate the core musculature, preventing tissue stagnancy without overexertion.
  3. Heat therapy — liberally using hot baths, showers, hot tubs, and heating pads.
  4. Eliminating perpetuating factors — the factors that tend to cause trigger points to form in the first place. We’ve already discussed a major one (stress and anxiety), but there are several others.

If the “confidence cure” is the most original and important idea in this tutorial, self-treatment of trigger points is the other original and important idea. On the one hand, trigger point therapy is actually fairly easy to do, but on the other hand, it can be surprisingly difficult to get good trigger point therapy from therapists. And, even when you can get good trigger point therapy, it’s usually too expensive to get enough of it. Just a little skill with self-treatment can make up the difference, either supplementing or completely replacing professional therapy. Time and again over the years, I have seen low back pain patients improve with relatively minor upgrades to their self-treatment methods. This is a critical principle for both patients and professionals to understand.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Limitations of trigger point therapy, and how to take advantage of them

This section is duplicated almost verbatim in the trigger points tutorial, because it’s particularly important: it answers the question, “Why self-treat?”

As mentioned repeatedly above, trigger point therapy is an unproven treatment — experimental medicine — and even anecdotally it is notorious for being hit or miss. If you do it yourself, you may be less effective than a professional might be, but at least you won’t go broke trying. It’s best not to pay upwards of a buck a minute for trial and error when you can experiment on yourself at no charge, safely.

This is the raison d’etre of this website, actually: when the professionals are nearly as unreliable as you are, you might as well save yourself instead of paying for professional shots in the dark. Obviously there are limitations to self-treatment — some spots you just can’t reach! — just as there are limitations to professional therapy. The only dramatic difference between professional care and self-treatment is the cost.

The beauty of trigger points is that you can use the worst things about them to your own advantage. They may be tricky and stubborn and weird, but you have time to mess around. You can wait. You can experiment. You can fiddle. For free. For years, if necessary — as long as there’s evidence that you’re gaining ground.

There are three basic problems with trigger point therapy, for both the pros and their patients:

  1. Locating trigger points can be tricky, and it’s hard to treat what you can’t find.
  2. Even when you’ve found trigger points, they don’t necessarily go away just because you squish ‘em, stretch ‘em, heat ‘em, or any of the other common treatment themes.
  3. And even if they do go away, they usually don’t stay away: trigger points have a nasty habit of coming back.

Fumbling around with diagnosis. Trigger points are not easy to confidently locate, and research has shown that even the professionals struggle to find them for you. At best, it’s possible but difficult; at worst, it’s a crapshoot.412 Thus, hunting for trigger points invariably involves expensive fumbling around. When I was a Registered Massage Therapist, I was painfully aware that $1.67 was flying out of my patient’s pocket every single minute as I hunted around for their trigger points — I ought to be damned good to justify that kind of expense, and the sad truth is that I wasn’t always, and I was supposedly unusually expert at it (the author of an entire book about back pain and trigger points).413

The right professional may be able to “fumble better,” and give patients good treatment ideas. But as your own patient, you definitely have an advantage: you literally have all day to find the right spot.

Fumbling around with treatment. And then there’s the mystery factor, the overall scientific cluelessness about why trigger points form in the first place — not what they are, but why they happen. Which means it’s impossible to be confident about exactly what flavour of treatment is going to make them go away. Professionals are definitely not privy to the magic trigger-point-begone formula, and while extensive hands-on experience undoubtedly leads to somewhat higher quality experimentation, it’s experimentation nevertheless. And — once again — the experimenting is expensive.414 But patients can and should experiment with different approaches willy nilly. As a patient doing self-treatment, you might or might not get results, but at least the insult of a great expense is not added to your injury.

Fumbling around with perpetuating factors. The third basic problem with trigger point therapy is that a trigger point comes back, like The Cat in the Hat. The forces that tended to lead to them in the first place routinely result in their resurgence. Even successful trigger point therapy is notoriously prone to being temporary. But once again, we snatch victory from the jaws of defeat thanks to the logic of self-treatment: if your benefits are going to be brief, better that they also be cheap!

It’s also up to patients to make changes in their lives that make them less prone to persistent trigger points. A good therapist may have excellent suggestions for things to try, but an educated patient is nearly as capable. How hard is it, really, to guess that your crappy, uncomfortable office chair may be the reason your trigger points just keep coming back? If stress seems to be a factor in the stubbornness of your muscle pain, that’s not particularly difficult to figure out — certainly not after doing a bunch of reading on this website. It’s also a deeply personal problem to solve, and the solution likely doesn’t have much to do with physical therapy or massage therapy.415

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Basic self-massage for low back trigger points

Photograph of a man sitting, reaching behind his back, and massaging his low back muscles.

Self-massage offers the best potential bang for buck of all treatments for back pain.

It is not difficult to apply pressure yourself to key trigger points in the low back, especially with the assistance of simple tools such as a tennis ball and other self-massage tools. For average cases of stubborn low back pain, a small investment in experimenting with applying pressure is surprisingly effective. People who’ve had intractable back pain for years may experience more relief than they ever have before. Some may even find that self-treatment of trigger points becomes the only thing they need over the long term to control their symptoms enough to feel “good enough,” if not actually completely symptom free.

Not every case goes quite that well, of course.

Some patients will need to experiment more and may find that self-massage is never a complete solution in itself. Some trigger points are definitely resistant to self-treatment, and there are fundamental limitations to self-treatment for trigger points, particularly in the low back. It’s an awkward area to work on yourself, and sometimes the tough, thick muscle of the low back requires much more pressure — and/or more accurate pressure — than you can possibly apply without assistance.

Nevertheless, it is well worth trying, and it is worth trying quite a bit, for quite a while. I would encourage every chronic low back pain sufferer to become well-versed in trigger points before giving up on self-treatment.

The following free articles explain in detail four of the classic, clinically important trigger points in the region of the low back. Although there is always more to learn, these four trigger points are more than enough for most beginners to work with for a long time.

For an easy case, literally just a single session consisting of a few moments of gentle rubbing can be enough. For slightly more difficult symptoms, a day or two of applying small but frequent doses of rubbing will usually do the trick. An investment of about a half dozen mini-treatments per day, each about 20–30 kneading strokes, can dramatically reduce pain and stiffness.

Here are a bunch more specific tips …

Rub in what way? For simplicity, either simply press on the trigger point directly and hold the pressure still, or apply very small kneading strokes, either circular or back and forth across the muscle fibres.

Rub how hard? The intensity of the treatment should be strong, but easy to live with. On a scale of 10 — where 1 is painless and 10 is intolerable — please aim for the 5–7 range, and err on the side of gentler at first.

What should it feel like? Pressure on a muscle knot should usually be clear and strong and satisfying; it should have a relieving, welcome quality. This is “good pain.” If you are wincing or gritting your teeth, you may need to be more gentle. You need to be able to relax.

What if it backfires? It probably won’t. But if you experience any negative reaction in the hours after treatment, simply ease up. In basic therapy, you can always count on trigger points adapting to stronger pressures over the course of a few days of regular treatment. If they don’t, either the problem isn’t really trigger points, or they are worse trigger points than you thought!

Rub how much? Massage each suspected trigger point for about 30 seconds. This is actually enough for many trigger points — especially if you have several that need attention! Five minutes is roughly the maximum that any trigger point will need at one time, but there is not really any limit — if rubbing the trigger point continues to feel good, feel free to keep going.

Rub how often? As long as you aren’t experiencing any negative reactions, you should massage a key trigger point at least once per day, and as often as a half dozen times per day.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

How do you know it’s working? Getting a trigger point to “release”

The goal of self-massage for trigger points is to achieve a “release.” What is trigger point “release” and what does it feel like? How do you measure success?

Trigger point release is the relaxation of the tightly contracted muscle tissue that the trigger point is made of. Unfortunately, this happy change in state may not be obvious, even when it occurs! The problem is that the tissue remains polluted with waste metabolites even after a successful release. Release may actually involve or even require damage to the tissue of the muscle knots. This means that it will probably still be sensitive to pressure, even if you’ve succeeded.

For beginners, the way to cope with this problem is to just not worry about it! Simply trust that you probably achieved a release, or a partial release, and then wait for the tissue to recover. Over the next several hours, if you were successful, you will notice a distinct reduction in symptoms — mission accomplished.

With easy trigger points, successful release is associated with “good pain” — that clear, strong, and satisfying sensation that is somehow both painful and yet also relieving.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Massage tools are indispensable

Ah, the humble tennis ball

Best buddy to the common muscle knot!

It is possible to massage your low back with your thumbs and fingers, or perhaps your fist, but it’s not especially easy or effective. The low back muscles are thick and tough. Although they are hypersensitive to pressure in some patients, in others they are “numb,” and require enormous pressures to achieve a satisfying and therapeutic pressure. Tools are therefore indispensable in self-massage for low back pain.

One of my clients once described an experience she’d had with an orthopedic specialist:

He didn’t know about using tennis balls for massage! He asked what helped my back pain, and I told him I always lie on a tennis ball. He looked at me like he was going to refer me to a psychiatrist! How can an orthopedic surgeon not know about the tennis ball thing? Doesn’t everyone know about the tennis ball thing?

Unfortunately, no: not everyone knows about the tennis ball thing. But it is one of the most time-honoured simple solutions for chronic muscle aches and pains, running a close second to the hot tub thing.

Indeed, a tennis ball is a handy tool for self-treatment of knots in your muscle.

The basic idea of tennis ball massage, or any massage with any kind of ball, is simply to trap the ball between your body and something else: usually the floor, but sometimes a wall, another body part, or creative options like the back of the couch or the bottom of the bathtub. The point is to use the ball to reach spots that you simply can’t get to with your hands, and every other kind of massage tool is a variation on this theme!

Tennis ball massage is usually the most useful in the muscles of the back and the hips: places you can actually lie down on the tennis ball, pinching it between your body and the floor or wall. Many other locations are awkward (especially for beginners), and you may find it difficult or impossible to apply pressure effectively.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The bath trick

From the Department of Why Didn’t I Think Of This Before: the bath trick! I discovered this long ago while working on my own muscle knots, trying to tame an episode of back pain, which seems to be a never-ending job — they are always under control, more or less, but also always threatening to come back.

This is what trigger points do, of course — they come back. It’s in their nature. And that’s why I’m always working on them, and always discovering new ways of doing it. It’s a creative challenge that never ends.

The Bath Trick

Run a hot bath, then trap a ball between your body & the bottom or back of the tub to rub your back muscles — your buoyancy allows for excellent control over moderate pressures.

The bath trick is a “together at last” trick: it came from combining two other classic tactics for releasing your own trigger points: the heat of a bath and the pressure of a ball. But the result is more than the sum of the parts, and it works better in some ways than anything else I’d come up with before. Suddenly I’m using the bath trick regularly myself and recommending it to readers.

  1. Simply run a hot bath …
  2. climb in and get nice and warm and comfortable …
  3. … and then bring in a ball! Trap the ball between your body and the bottom or the back of the tub, and cheerfully crush your trigger points with relieving pressure.

Ah!

The bath trick works particularly well because the pressure is easy to control.

Often people find that the full weight of their body trapping a tennis ball against the floor is simply too much — the pressure is too intense, and they can’t achieve a relieving sensation. But in the bath, you are much lighter! You have much better control and a moderate intensity of pressure.

While the heat relaxes you, your buoyancy in the water allows finely tuned control over moderate pressure on your trigger points. Applying a little more or less pressure is as simple as rising up in the water a little, or submerging more of yourself.

The KONG® brand dog ball is <em>perfect</em> for the bath trick.

The KONG® brand dog ball is perfect for the bath trick.

What sort of ball should you use? Some balls are better for the bath than others. Tennis balls, usually so useful for self-treatment, are not especially good for the bath trick. But a KONG® brand rubber ball is perfect — not the pyramidal KONG with the hole through the center (although it could work), but preferably the KONG ball, which is made with the same dense rubber. They don’t get soaked, and they have a nice neutral buoyancy — they don’t sink or bob up to the surface — so it’s nice and easy to move them around underwater, trapping them right where you want between your body and the bathtub.

By contrast, wet tennis balls are kind of a pain outside the bath, and they like to pop up to the surface!

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Can you damage your nerves when self-massaging?

Is this a realistic scenario? Can you damage nerves with self-massage? Here’s the executive summary for this section: no. Case closed.

Fortunately, it is nearly impossible to damage your nerves with self-massage. In my years of clinical experience, I have never known of anyone doing it.

Nerves are well-sheltered from pressure at all locations behind and beside the spine. It’s possible to compress them enough to feel it, but it is nearly impossible to compress them enough to cause damage. Nor are nerves naturally sensitive to pressure. Most nerves, most of the time, can be compressed firmly without producing any symptoms whatsoever.

However, there are physiological circumstances where nerves can be more sensitive — such as low back pain. Lumbar nerve roots may be irritable due to the stagnancy of the tissue around them, or from exposure to the chemicals released from a ruptured intervertebral disc, to name two examples. In such cases, you may feel some nerve pain or other strange symptoms with relatively minor pressures in the low back. As long as the sensations are minor (no worse than your typical symptoms), it’s no cause for alarm. If your nerves were at any risk, you would feel much more severe, highly off-putting electrical or zappy pains long before you were at any risk of actually damaging the nerve.

Even if you did persist — increasing your pressure in spite of such symptoms, which you are no more likely to do than you are to stick your hand into a jar of irritable scorpions — any injury you would do would most likely be minor. Nerves can recover from an enormous amount of abuse, up to and including being mangled in nasty accidents, or being pinched hard for years. For instance, many people who have truly severe carpal tunnel syndrome — years of crippling median nerve impingement — recover just fine once pressure on the nerve is finally relieved by surgery.

In the rare event that you might cause a nerve injury, it would probably result in only annoying but trivial symptoms that would take a few days to resolve, or perhaps a few weeks at the worst. But I have literally never heard of this happening by self-massage — it takes a lot of pressure, and it hurts too much as you approach the point of injury to actually get there.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Don’t hesitate to recruit amateur help

Do not underestimate the value of amateur assistance! A creative and generous amateur can be just as effective a trigger point therapist as the average professional, certainly for easy cases — and even for difficult ones!

At least half the challenge of trigger points is simply recognizing their importance in the first place — getting to the point where you accept that trigger points are your problem, or a large part of it. Many people need a professional to help them get to that point, or a tutorial like this, or a popular book like Clair Davies’ The Trigger Point Therapy Workbook.

I have often worked with patients who seem to have readily treatable cases of muscle pain in the low back, but who have been given dozens of different diagnoses over the years, many of them scary. Such people are unlikely to believe, at first, that simple trigger point therapy might be all they ever needed. And no amateur can help them through that process: only a confident professional, putting the diagnostic possibilities in perspective, can guide a frustrated, cynical, and confused patient to that awareness.

But once you are there … the therapy itself can be quite easy! So easy that even an amateur can do it. As you’ve seen in the preceding sections, trigger point therapy is not difficult — most of the time it’s just a little rubbing.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

The evidence for massaging back pain

I am going to go into considerable depth here, because that is what I do, but in this case there is an extreme contrast between my lengthy delving and the simple bottom line. It’s all going to boil down to this:

error! insufficient data!

We just don’t know if massage works for back pain (let alone massage with a focus on trigger points). At best, what we have leans towards “promising,” for short term results especially. At worst, it’s already over: massage doesn’t seem to work any better than anything else, or it already would have shown up in the research (limited and imperfect as it is). But I suspect the truth is that we simply haven’t studied it well enough in the right way yet. I am not aware of a single study of massage for back pain that doesn’t have at least one serious flaw, and none at all that have studied massage the way I’d like to see it done.

Until we have better research, any kind of massage for back pain is experimental medicine.

If all that mattered was the bottom line, I could safely end this chapter right now. But I have to go on! It’s my job to fully understand the research, and “show my work” — you parted with $20 to read this not just for the bottom line, but because I promised to go all the way down the rabbit hole, and to make it as interesting as possible.

But rarely in my work has so much rabbit-hole-diving come to so little.

For many years I published my opinion on the efficacy of massage without the support of any good evidence. For a science writer, that was an uncomfortable position. This book devotes a lot of words to debunking treatment methods of dubious value, while massage is one of the only therapies that I have recommended. Although I’ve always been clear that it’s no miracle cure, I’ve recommended it as a good therapeutic option, “because reasons.”416

So, am I a hypocrite with a pet theory and a double standard, supporting one treatment on the basis of insufficient evidence while condemning others for the same thing? Or does the evidence back me up?

It’s probably closer to the hypocrite side of things. 😮

In my defense, for many years the best review of the science that was available, by Furlan et al., had a cautiously optimistic conclusion, declaring that “massage is beneficial.”417 Furthermore, of the dozen or so low quality studies they had reviewed so far, there was a reassuring pattern: the most positive studies also seemed to be the best studies. Always a good sign.

But then, in 2015, Furlan et al. added another dozen studies to the pool of data, and changed their tune: now they have “very little confidence that massage is an effective treatment for LBP.”418 And nor should they. Although there are scraps of good news, the evidence damns massage with faint praise — just like all other “promising” back pain treatments.

Furlan et al.

 … in 2008: “massage is beneficial”

 … in 2015: “very little confidence that massage is an effective treatment”

The change from optimism to pessimism is fascinating, and caused some angst in the small community of massage therapists who pay attention to research. Dr. Christopher Moyer explains the flip-flop in an interview for Massage & Fitness Magazine:

I think it is more that they are circumspect than pessimistic. Speaking as a scientist, we are very careful to guard against declaring a finding if there is even a small risk of it being a false positive. We never want to say ‘we’ve found something’ and later have it turn out we were wrong when more data comes in. So, I think they are hewing to scientific norms in this regard, and I do not fault them for that; it is important to be careful in science.

In this case, they may have regretted saying “massage is beneficial” based on inadequate evidence in the past, because they did indeed have to walk their optimism back as more data came in. But was it because the results were less positive? Or just that the evidence is such junk?

Garbage in, garbage out

Despite their bottom-line pessimism, Furlan et al. also reported good results: massage actually appeared to be better than control treatments, especially in the short term. In fact, the results sound so positive that you might actually wonder why the authors “have very little confidence that massage is an effective treatment for LBP.” From the plain language summary of the paper:

Massage was better than inactive controls for pain and function in the short-term, but not in the long-term follow-up. Massage was better than active controls for pain both in the short and long-term follow-ups.

But the results are also not positive enough. A skeptic would argue that massage should have been more better, a stronger effect — not just slightly better.

And the evidence comes from pretty junky science. There are only a few dozen studies of massage for back pain at all, and most of them suck. Although Furlan et al.’s 2015 meta-analysis was competently conducted, it suffered badly from the “garbage in, garbage out” problem, and the authors knew it:

The quality of the evidence for all comparisons [studies] was graded “low ” or “very low” which means that we have very little confidence in these results. This is because most of the included studies were small and had methodological flaws.

A bunch of low quality studies with a high risk of bias — conducted by people with a dog in the fight, people who were probably trying to prove that massage works — should be able to produce much clearer evidence of efficacy. If the results are weak, then those results damn massage with faint praise. (That’s what I have concluded about many other treatments.)

But sometimes the results are weak not because there were no strong ones to find, but because researchers didn’t look in the right place. In fact, I do not think these results damn massage with faint praise.

Comparing apples to orange cars: 25 completely different massage therapy studies

The studies Furlan et al. studied were all over the map. It would be hard to imagine a more eclectic group of experiments that are ostenibly about the same thing. In my opinion, this was the biggest problem here. There’s not much point in analyzing pooled data about different things: you need to compare apples-to-apples. This review was saddled with comparing apples to … orange cars. Almost everything was different in every study:

  • practitioner credentials and experience were different
  • techniques used as a comparison to massage were different
  • experimental methodologies were different
  • types of back pain were different (not bad, but definitely some)
  • and, worst of all, the massage techniques were very different

What a mess!

For instance, massage techniques in these studies ranged from traditional Thai massage to “one 30-minute session of deep cross-friction massage” to “acupuncture massage” to — *facepalm* — reflexology. Three papers used reflexology! That disappoints me: reflexology is not massage therapy, it’s quackery that is superficially similar to massage, a foot rub with snake oil. Even if reflexology actually worked, it still wouldn’t be representative of “massage” — massage for back pain almost never involves reflexology. Including these 3 reflexology studies was like throwing some dead fish into the data pool. It seems almost like sabotage, a guarantee that at least 10% of the evidence was out in left field, a glaring flaw in an otherwise excellent paper.

Meanwhile, where is “trigger point therapy”? Even loosely defined as massage that focusses on sensitive spots, trigger point massage was almost totally absent from any of the research, and wasn’t the focus of a single study. And yet I think it’s the most likely kind of massage to make a difference in low back pain, which is why it was the primary approach I used in my own massage therapy practice for many years. I’ll return to the evidence for this kind of massage below.

Clearly, this was not a tidy apples-to-apples review, not by a long shot. Most of the studies had almost little in common except that they were all experimenting with some kind of massage-like therapy for some kind of back pain.

Sometimes “more study needed” is a bullshit cover for the fact that the research so far has simply failed to produce the good news that someone wants. The lack of strong, clear positive results is the result.

But sometimes more study really is needed, and I think this is a great example. The low quality and great variety in what was studied undermine the results so much that they mean almost nothing at all, which is why I don’t think the results damn massage with faint praise. The right research simply hasn’t been done yet. Furlan et al.’s review, as competent as it is, simply cannot answer the question: garbage in, garbage out.

“Cherry picking” the best studies

Let’s leave Furlan et al. behind now. What do the best studies say? What if I do my own review, deliberately leaving out things like sketchy trials of reflexology, and picking only the studies that I think are the most relevant to what I think constitutes good massage therapy for back pain? Can I cherry pick my way to a happy ending?

Let’s see what happens …

One of the best studies, a Canadian experiment conducted by Michele Preyde way back in 2000,419 was a test of “comprehensive massage therapy” as delivered by well-trained Ontario therapists,420 in six sessions in a month for 25 cases of sub-acute low back pain (non-chronic, but not brand new cases either). Comprehensive massage included remedial exercise prescription and posture education (as would typically be provided by massage therapists in Ontario). This treatment regimen was compared to massage alone, remedial exercise and posture education alone, or some useless laser therapy. Massage alone had “considerable benefit,” just enough to be considered clinically significant; adding exercise prescriptions (and posture education, but that probably wasn’t a difference maker) improved on those results even more, pushing them comfortably into clinical significance.

Booyah! That passes the impress-me test, and I am rarely impressed.

But for all that, Preyde’s nice little study has a critical flaw: the frustrebo effect, a “frustrated placebo,” caused by a lack of blinding. That is, everyone recruited for this study knew full well that it was a massage study … and so the folks assigned to the non-massage group were likely frustrated by that, which can cause a negative placebo effect. People love massage, and being left out of it in this experiment would have been a bummer (plus they’re also suffering and actually hoping for help). And low back pain is notoriously sensitive to expectations! So this is a recipe for statistical disaster for the study: the massage patients are happier and the non-massage patients are less happy, and that could easily skew the results enough to explain away the modest benefits that Preyde supposedly found. And that would actually covert it into a negative study, finding confirmation of no effect of massage for back pain. And that was the sad conclusion reached by Dr. Lloyd Oppel in a short response paper for the Canadian Medical Association Journal: “this paper’s most powerful findings indicate a lack of effect for massage therapy when compared with nonmassage controls.”421

A frustrebo effect caused by a lack of blinding is also the critical flaw in the next notable study, a much bigger and higher profile one. It’s possible that no study of massage has ever avoided this problem — none I’m aware of. The frustrebo effect is a great demonstration of how tricky science is.

The largest ever controlled trial of massage for low back pain was published in 2011. (A separate article analyzes it in detail. See Massage Therapy Kinda, Sorta Works for Back Pain.) Unfortunately, the results were not encouraging, despite the fact that the authors thought so …

Massage therapy may be effective for treatment of chronic back pain, with benefits lasting at least 6 months.

A strongly positive summary, barely tempered by the word “may.” It would have been easy — so easy — for me to just take their word for it, to accept that interpretation, and to add it to my list of references supporting my enthusiasm for massage. Unfortunately, I could not help but notice that their evidence does not really support such a sunny conclusion. In fact, their data showed that the benefits of massage were minor to begin with, and barely detectable after six months. Worse still, there’s that lack of blinding thing again. They concede the flaw but fail to acknowledge its serious implications: if anything, as with Michele Preyde’s study, it flips the story, from good news to a depressing evidence of absence.

Never in a million years would I have summarized the way they did. I would have said, “A lot of expensive massage produced minor, temporary benefits at best, and maybe none at all.” That seems like a much more intellectually honest description!

So that’s two studies now that start out seeming like good news that any massage enthusiast could easily cite to show that “massage works for back pain” — but which actually prove no such thing. Is there any point in continuing? Is there any other good study worth “cherry picking”?

Not really, no. There are other candidates, but they all have major issues. I’ve made my point: it’s actually not possible to cite any evidence that is clearly, unambiguously promising. Not with integrity.

Maybe if we poke the trigger points?

Maybe! But don’t ask science — it’s playing hard-to-get here. Just as I’ve analyzed the massage-for-back-pain evidence in this tutorial ad nauseam, I get dorky in detail about the science of trigger point massage in that tutorial. Here’s just the bottom line …

Trigger point massage is effectively still 100% experimental. It has rarely been directly tested and it has never been done well422 and never for back pain specifically.423 As of late 2017, I’m aware of just 16 studies worth knowing about — if you keep your standards low — and all of them have serious flaws, all show signs of a high risk of bias, all claim to be positive while being contradicted to at least some degree by their own actual data.424

If you squint optimistically, you could call the best of this evidence promising. You could say that where there’s smoke, there’s fire. But it’s like the smoke from last night’s campfire — more of a smokey smell than a smoke where any fire might be hiding.

But dial up even a mild cynical impulse, and the evidence collectively looks more like a damning failure to produce any clearly good news.

Mostly there’s just no conclusion at all. We simply don’t know if it works, for back pain or anything else. Boo.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Heat and ice both provide good bang for buck, but err on the side of heat

Backs seem to love heat. Heat is one of the only things that reliably takes the edge off. The anecdotal "evidence" of this is overwhelming, while the science is almost nonexistent — especially for chronic low back pain. But the nearly universal human experience of being soothed by heat should not be dismissed. This is a useful tool, especially because it’s relatively cheap, easy, and safe.

Ceaveat! Burns happen! Although mostly quite safe, I’ve seen a few minor thermotherapy burns in my career (but never inflicted one). My own wife has managed to burn herself with overzealous use of a heating pad.

Heat may be a useful tool, but it’s also extremely unlikely to actually cure anything. Indeed, it never seems to help much more than a stiff drink. One of the few decent studies that’s ever been done on this topic disappointingly suggested that heat worked no better than ice, and neither of them worked all that well: they both provided only modest benefits.425 It’s not a great study, but a evidence of a modest therapeutic effect is better than nothing, and there are likely situations where heat helps more.

For heating the low back, use just about anything that lets you deliver heat directly and in a controlled way: heated gel packs, heating pads, Thermophores (a special heating pad), towels saturated in hot water, and so on.

The “Thermophore” electric heating pads from Battle Creek are high quality heating pads for the rehab industry that have gradually become consumer products as well. They are thick, cozy, large, and… moist? Moisture condenses on the fleece cover, which they call their “Moist-Sure™ technology.” Cheezy trademark, but it does what it says on the tin.

Full-body heating in immersive settings such as hot tubs, showers, saunas, and steam rooms use heat to treat low back pain and may work because of their stress-busting powers. To the extent that low back pain is a stress-aggravated condition — perhaps quite a lot — then soothing hot soaks may be more meaningful for low back pain patients. Unsurprisingly, there’s a lack of scientific research on this topic. See Hot Baths for Injury & Pain for tips on getting the most out of your bathtub or Jacuzzi — believe it or not, many people do not really know how to take a hot bath, and I have a bunch of tips that can help you wring more therapeutic value out of the experience.

The curious case of the Hydrocollator

For decades, Hydrocollator steam packs or hot packs have been a staple in the rehab business. No self-respecting therapeutic or physical therapy clinic would be caught dead without one: a hot water tank, roughly the size of a trashcan, filled with a set of heavy, clay-filled heating pads, keeping them toasty warm and ready for use. They produce a very moist heat, because they are soaking wet (but the clay retains the moisture, so they aren't excessively drippy).

Hydrocollators are an interesting case study in tradition versus science in rehab. They have a distinct old-timey vibe. They entered the marketplace in 1947, when they were first sold by the Chattanooga Pharmaceutical Company (now Chattem). They remain for sale today, with tanks ranging from a few hundred dollars to thousands (seriously).

On the one side of the equation, this product can easily be defended (or forgiven) as a pleasant and convenient way to deliver the best possible version of a classic folk remedy to back pain patients. What’s the big deal? Let people enjoy things! Seriously, I’m not being snarky here. If I had a home big enough to have a bathroom with some spa features, I’d probably want one of these things around — one of the smaller models, anyway — because heavy moist heat is just nice.

Photo of the interior of a hydrocollator tank, showing one steam pack being pulled out the hot water by a metal hook. The sides of the tank are steel or aluminum, and there are several vertical racks in the hot water that resemble oven racks.

The interior of a Hydrocollator tank. It doesn’t really look like luxury, but these things really are quite pleasant.

On the other side of the equation …

The continued popularity of this product obviously has a lot to do with tradition, momentum, and — most obnoxiously — business model, because it’s one of the most passive of all the “passive therapies,” and it’s literally valuable for therapists to apply steam packs to patients while they they go work with other patients, a practice pattern that has been gradually going extinct. Other than “it feels nice,” which it clearly does for most people, almost all of the other assumptions underlying this usage are rather dubious,426 there is basically zero science to support any of it, and the benefit is clearly minor at best.

And so perhaps it’s rather embarrassing for the physical therapy profession that they were ever used, let alone that they continue to be widely used.

And what about ice?

One of the most common issues for low back pain patients is confusion about ice versus heat. Heat is almost always preferable, because it is what almost all back pain patients prefer. Low back pain patients are usually heat seekers — but there are exceptions!

Ice should be used on the back only by patients who clearly prefer that (for whatever reason), or when there is definitely a fresh, superficial muscle injury (such as a true tear, when a significant exertion causes a sudden onset of strong pain). Ice will not usually do any harm, and context certainly matters: if you like the idea of ice or just believe (for whatever reason) that ice will help, like the patients in the ER study mentioned above, it will probably help.

But ice should never be imposed on a reluctant patient, because it can absolutely backfire. Most people clearly prefer to heat their low back pain, and a few have negative reactions to ice, when it’s unwanted. Consider this “chilling” example of an “ice backfire”:

I was receiving a pleasant massage from an RMT for a low back or sacroiliac joint problem. Everything was going well, and I was feeling quite a bit better, when suddenly she put an ice pack directly on my skin. She gave no warning at all, just put it on. It was so startling and unpleasant that my back muscles started to spasm, and all the good she’d done was completely reversed. It was a disaster! Obviously, I never went back there ...

anonymous client

Talk about terrible technique and bedside manner! Alas, ice is often prescribed and delivered carelessly for the patient’s own good, without regard for their preferences. And that’s exactly when it tends to backfire, and why I want patients to feel free to choose!

For more on ice, heat, and ice vs. heat, see also:

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Act normal! Rest minimally and strategically, while maintaining as much normal activity as you can

Normal activity levels — or as close to normal as you can get — are the best general course of action in response to most acute episodes of back pain.

It doesn’t sound like much, does it?

But there’s more to “act normal” and light general exercise than first meets the eye: finding and loitering in the activity Goldilocks zone is actual pretty good challenge. One study found that ordinary activity got better results than either bed rest or exercise.427 Some rest may well be appropriate, but it should be minimal and tactical.

Rest in peace, bed rest

Bed rest, once a standard recommendation for anyone with acute back pain, is just not a good idea.428 According to an updated review of the science in 2005, bed rest may even cause more pain429 — ouch!

Unfortunately, I still hear from readers who have been told that they must get flat and stay that way. This just exacerbates the idea of spinal fragility, reinforces the fear of disability, does little or nothing for pain, and results in making stagnant and understimulated tissues even more so.

However, it’s sensible to beware of activities that obviously aggravate your low back pain, and avoid them. What if your “normal activity” is unhealthy? I made it clear above to beware of advice to entirely quit favourite activities, but taking breaks from your routine may make sense when you suspect certain activities are part of the problem. An irritating activity or position — or even stagnancy — may contribute to the vicious cycle of back pain, and so it may be helpful to remove it from the equation briefly, experimentally, to see if it makes a difference.

The main challenge here is to decide whether any activity avoidance is actually called for in your particular case of back pain. How much do you think your back pain was caused by a specific activity or continues to be aggravated by it? That can be surprisingly difficult to figure out. The more confident you are about that, the more you should consider taking a break. Just remember that connection should be obvious — anything less than obvious, and it’s doubtful that the activity is actually a key factor.

One way I often see this thinking going wrong: blaming a harmless activity. Usually it’s a case of rapid onset where the patient doesn’t actually know what triggered the pain (that outta nowhere phenomenon again), and is trying to fill in that blank with whatever activity they happened to be doing at the time (or earlier that day or week.) “I was playing a lot of golf, so it must be the golf, therefore I must take a rest from the terribly stressful activity that is golf.” Probably not. If you are blaming an activity that does not seem particularly hard on the back, then it probably isn’t.430

If the need for rest isn’t obvious, then you probably don’t need much rest. Rest is obviously warranted in the case of the construction worker who had to repeat a particularly back-beating chore several hundred times in a row, with the pain getting worse and worse as he goes. It’s obviously a good idea in the case of the farmer who planted 400 yards of eggplant seedlings. It’s obvious in the case of the bodybuilder doing entirely too many dead lifts but still pushing on when he knew better. And it’s obvious in the case of the office worker putting in 12-hour days in an ill-fitting chair.

But it’s rarely that obvious. In the majority of cases where getting worn out is the main problem, rest is an equally obvious solution, and that patient never needs to read this book. For most back pain patients, it’s a more complicated mix, and resting is a little trickier — possibly helpful, but also potentially harmful. Consider a more complicated and typical example:

I had a client whose back pain was probably aggravated by a distinctive slightly flexed position that he assumes at work, a simple postural over-exertion. I advised him to continue to be an active person, getting any kind of exercise that pleased him. I also suggested that he find another way of positioning himself at work for a while and to make a point of taking some shorter days and maybe a long weekend at the same time that he worked on other therapeutic angles. In short, I told him to rest temporarily from that awkward posture, but not rest in general. I think it would actually have been quite harmful to tell this patient to permanently change his working posture — to advise total rest from that posture. Why? Wouldn’t some posture improvement just be wise? Well, maybe … but it can be extremely difficult to change postures, and meanwhile I didn’t want him to feel bullied by his back pain into thinking that he couldn’t handle something as innocuous as a slightly stooped posture.

It may benefit you to work on a specific posture, but there is also virtue in taking a stand and saying, “I should really be able to sit this way without it being a big problem.” And that is absolutely possible! Many people tolerate their lousy postures just fine. So for this patient, I quite deliberately placed the emphasis on getting some exercise and some temporary caution with the working posture.

Warnings to completely rest from or avoid a posture can sound like this to the patient: “Your back is so weak and pathetic that you can’t handle sitting. You are screwed if you don’t stop sitting in a particular way. You’re in awful pain now, and you always will be unless you can figure out how to pull off the seemingly impossible challenge of a permanent change in your posture. You might succeed for a while, but chances are you will backslide … and it’ll hurt!”

I’m exaggerating a little to make a point, of course. But I have seen quite a few patients and readers who have somehow — in most cases, they got the idea from a therapist with good intentions — developed a long-term paranoid hypervigilance about avoiding (“resting from”) activity X or factor Y. Even if they are out of pain, I’d call that a failure as far as quality of life goes. All this is bad enough, but it’s far worse still if the posture wasn’t actually even a relevant factor to begin with — which is definitely a possibility.

Ideally, you should use rest cautiously and temporarily to break a cycle of pain-causing-pain. However, remember that you should be able to resume activities that were previously problematic. And that you should focus on choosing to “act normal” — it’s an excellent, evidence-based strategy, especially for acute episodes of back pain where there’s no obvious connection with any particular activity. However, you can do even better! If your normal activity isn’t healthy, how about simulating or imitating a “normal” activity level? You can proactively target the affected tissues with the best possible exercise strategy: mobilizations …

Bourne concentrated on rest and mobility. From somewhere in his forgotten past he understood that recovery depended upon both and he applied rigid discipline to both.

The Bourne Identity, by Robert Ludlum, p. 137

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Massage with movement and life in the Goldilocks zone — light and general exercise

Do do do do, do do do do, do do do do …

Can you exercise to help beat chronic back pain in the low back? Maybe. But you must enter …

The Goldilocks Zone.

One of the pillars of trigger point therapy is regular, easy exercise. Exercise intensity that is “just right.” Just enough stimulation to keep your tissues warm and happy … but not enough to irritate them.

Anyone struggling with back pain must (must, must) embrace life in the Goldilocks Zone. This is probably particularly true in the low back. The low back is almost never adequately stimulated by normal activity alone. Our modern lives do not lend themselves to moderate, regular stimulation of our back joints and muscles. We tend to live at the extremes, either standing or sitting for hours at work and at home, or pushing our limits in athletic recreation.

For maximum back health, you must regularly but gently stimulate your body. Weekend warriors need to let the ski slopes go for a while and take up a walking habit instead. Hardcore power yoga practitioners need to switch to a gentler class. Office workers have to learn to take regular breaks from the chair. Gym rats have to ease up on the “reps to failure.” And so on.

You can easily take the idea of the Goldilocks Zone and run with it yourself. It’s not hard to understand the principle. But the idea that absolutely everyone needs — the perfect embodiment of the Goldilocks Zone — is the idea of mobilizations.

Mobilizations are rhythmic, repeated movements that alternately stretch and contract musculature and other soft tissue — massaging your tissues with movement. I prescribe at least one or two key mobilizations to nearly every client, usually in preference to stretching. Mobilizations are more neurologically interesting than stretching, and they stimulate more metabolic activity in the tissue while remaining gentle. They are more practical and efficient than stretching in many ways, especially because they can affect more tissues more quickly. They also constitute both a better warm up and a better warm down for more intense activity.

Immobility is a major part of the vicious cycle of low back pain, but also one of the easier ones to break — once you start to trust that your back is not fragile, and there is no danger in using it.

Movement inspires confidence: as you move more and more, both you and your overprotective nervous system learn to trust the health of your spine again. I strongly encourage clients to emphasize the mental part of this exercise, treating every repetition of a movement like a message to your back: “everything’s cool, everything’s good, no problems here … ”

Mobilizations teach the back to behave like a back again, to do spinal things, to do what backs do.

Mobilizations stimulate low back tissues with much greater safety and precision than other common approaches to exercise therapy for the low back. By using your pain-free range of motion systematically, you can provide much more variety of stimulation to your low back musculature than you ever could simply by getting up from your chair regularly to run errands around the office — and, as long as you only do whatever movement is mostly painless and only in modest doses, it’s also nearly impossible to hurt yourself.

For more general information about mobilizations, see Mobilize! Dynamic joint mobility drills are an alternative to stretching, a way to “massage with movement”

Some nice mobilizations for the low back…

Stick-Yer-Bum-Out

Summary: Stick your bum backwards and out to one side while twisting the spine. This reaches deep into one side of the low back. Then reverse the motion, and do the other side.

This unusual mobilization reaches the low back and gluteus maximus in a way that no other exercise can, stimulating that hard-to-reach spot in the very bottom “corner” of the low back, just above the dimple. Stick your bum out as though you are going to sit down on a stool that is well behind you and off to one side. Your knees must bend. Round your low back like the top of a ball, lean your torso in the other direction (i.e. bum goes left, torso goes right), and twist your shoulders to face back towards the middle. (The further you lean and twist, the further the stretch will “reach” into your upper back.) To conclude, stand up and lean back a bit, clenching your gluteal and low back muscles firmly. Now alternate from side to side: left, clench, right, clench, left, clench, etc.

Standing Forward Bend

Summary: Straight from yoga: touch your toes and then stand up with a rolling motion of the spine.

Stand with your feet shoulder width apart and toes pointed slightly inwards, knees unlocked — this is not a hamstring stretch. Breathe steadily for the duration of the exercise. Begin by lowering your chin to your chest and continue to flex your intervertebral joints one at a time. The lower parts of the spine should remain erect for as long as possible. Continue until your entire upper body is hanging from your hips, and make sure your head is a relaxed dead-weight at the end of the spine. At this point, you have two choices for getting back upright: for the first 3-5 repetitions, you should (a) squat down, slowly straighten your back, and then stand up using the strength of your legs; but once you are warmed up a bit, you can (b) simply reverse the spinal movement instead, rolling up instead of down, lifting the vertebrae back into place one by one, starting with the low back and moving upwards.

Deep Gluteal Mobilization

Summary: Sit, cross one leg over the other, and lean forward. Then spread the feet and push the knees in and down. Far superior to a common physiotherapy stretch prescribed for sciatica and piriformis syndrome.

Starting from a seated position, place your ankle on the stretch side over your knee on the other side. Let your lifted knee relax downwards for a moment, and then begin to lean forward from your pelvis. Avoid simply slumping forward, which is useless. Visualize pushing your belly button between your legs. Now do this on the other side, to even yourself out, even if you have symptoms only on one side. Now, to complete the mobilization for both sides, place your feet widely on the floor, and drop your knees straight down towards the floor.

Mobilizations are not a life sentence. I recommend that you do them only on an as-needed basis. When you begin to self-treat low back pain, begin conservatively with only 10–20 repetitions of a given mobilization exercise per day, but then build quickly up to 40+/day of the most relieving exercises for a few days. Thereafter, you can do all the exercises “as needed” — whenever you think they might feel good or useful. The idea here is to do a lot up front, but not much later on. In my experience, almost no one is good at doing therapeutic exercises for long, so I don’t prescribe them as a constitutional or preventative: just a bunch of them at first to kick-start tissues that have forgotten normal function, and then trailing off rapidly. But they are always in your bag of tricks if you need them again.

Many people should use these exercises when taking “microbreaks” at work. They are an ideal way to quickly and efficiently stimulate stagnant low back tissues, to keep your back in the Goldilocks zone. More about microbreaking below in the section about managing low back pain when you work in a chair all day.

But even if you aren’t a “chair warrior,” microbreaking is a useful concept. For instance, mail carriers also experience a kind of stagnancy in their low back pain tissues, a sensory boredom with the same slight low back muscular contractions with every step, all day, every day. Mail carriers can benefit just as much from stopping every three blocks to take a microbreak of 30 seconds and do three standing forward bends or “stick yer bum out” mobilizations.

Another intriguing option is passive mobilization — that is, getting moved by something instead of moving yourself. However, this needs a partner or equipment. In the traction section (next) I give the example of spinal “jostlers” (like the Back2Life tool) that offer a little mobilizing and a little traction simultaneously — a fine, simple idea.

And finally, of course, get your heart rate up in whatever way pleases you

In addition to stimulating the spine and back specifically, you should also just stimulate everything and anything whenever you can — without obviously irritating your back. Some of your are groaning already, but bear with me: this is a good-news-bad-news thing.

The bad news is that this isn’t going to fix anyone’s back pain in the short term.

The good news is that it’s a fantastic investment, and it takes amazingly little exercise to be way better than nothing. You do not have to become “hardcore.” You do not have to do crunches or pull-ups like you’re in a training montage. You do not have to embrace the stereotypical lifestyle of the amateur athlete.

Just take the stairs more often. Jog two blocks now and then. Seriously. That’s all we’re talking about here. And this is evidence-based advice.

Photo of a set of indoor stairs. Each stair is labelled with a cumulative calorie-count in half calories increments: 0.5 calories, 1.0 calories, 1.5 calories, up to 6.5 at the top of the photo.

The calorie-counting stairs (click to zoom). Stairs are the most ubiquitous, accessible “gym equipment” in the world

Skeptics have been pointing out for years that exercise may not “fix” anything but a lack of fitness, and a lack of fitness is not a cause pain. Not directly anyway. Not in the short term. But exercise in general? As a “tonic” that improves health in a many ways, almost certainly boosting your “immunity” to chronic pain or failed healing ? Exercise remains rather amazing for that. It probably does improve your odds in all kinds of good ways over time.

For instance, we know that we can reduce inflammation with long-term daily exercise. Klasson et al is a good 2022 trial linking long-term daily physical activity to clear reductions in systemic inflammation (as measured by major biomarkers).431 They found “evidence for both systemic metabolic effects via thyroid hormones and in specific systems via reduced inflammation and immune cell counts.” This is a promising clue for many people with unexplained chronic pain who feel “inflamed” a lot. Some of them probably are, and doing as much exercise as you can is one of the only evidence-based strategies for solving that over the long haul (along with avoiding junk food and sleep loss and other lifestyle medicine basics).

And also for instance, we know that mini workouts, short but strong, are absurdly good bang for your exercise buck. Ahmadi et al did one of those morbid mortality studies, a big one cross-referencing deaths with fitness gadget data for tens of thousands of people.432 People died less if they did frequent small doses of vigorous exercise — less than 2-minute sessions, and only one or two per day. Intense and regular, but just little blasts of action. The benefits became measurable at 15-minutes per week, and continued to improve up to a total of an hour per week (or 4 mini workouts per day).

Four mini workouts per day sounds easy, almost like a slam dunk — and it definitely is easier than getting to the gym for 30-minutes per day. But it’s trickier than it sounds! I have tried to hit this goal a few times in recent years, and always failed. It’s a tricky habit for me to build.

(Note that “all-cause mortality” definitely overlaps with the “reduced systemic inflammation” measured by Klasson et al. taming inflammation was probably partly how mortality was reduced.)

Photo of a grey tabby cat climbing concrete stairs.

Take the stairs!

Every chance you get, as often as your body can handle it, for the rest of your life, with or without any current back pain. Every stair climb is a gift of a minute or two of exercise. Little bits of oomph won’t “fix” back pain any more than regular putting $100 in a savings account will fix poverty… but they add up & the benefits start to reinforce each other. Over the long term, it’s a truly great way improving your odds of minimizing any kind of pain.

General exercise for back pain isn’t “random” or “miscellaneous”

In scientific studies of back pain, "general exercise" has often been shown to compete just fine more specific and allegedly "corrective" exercises. This has resulted in a common misconception that any exercise will do, just as long as you get moving. Todd Hargrove:

General exercise isn’t “random” exercise. It is exercise that a patient chooses to do, and that choice often involves a screening process that may have some wisdom to it. People are more likely to choose exercises that are non-painful, fun, that fit into their schedules, that their friends do, and that relate to meaningful functional goals. Exercise that meets this criteria is far more likely to help with pain than some exercise chosen at random. And maybe more likely than exercises prescribed by movement therapists.

Great advice.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Strain-counterstrain (AKA positional release): find a neutral, comfy position and rest there

Resting peacefully in a position of comfort for a minute or two may ease chronic low back pain for much longer. There are a variety of techniques that exploit this idea in different ways, but “strain-counterstrain” therapy is the most common label for treating pain essentially just by positioning limbs for patients. It’s kind of a strange name.433 Most massage therapists call it “positional release.” I’ll mostly use SCS here. It has a surprisingly long history, which I discuss in more detail in the SCS chapter of the trigger points book.

We have here yet another odd treatment approach of unknown and probably low value at best. And also free. And also particularly harmless, since the point of it is to do almost literally nothing, like a form of meditation. At worst, SCS is just a comfortable, peaceful waste of time.

SCS is almost comically simplistic, but I can put a modern pain science spin on it: if it works, it’s probably because it exploits the principle that chronic pain often has a strong component of excessive sensory alarm (sensitization, previously discussed). I wouldn’t recommend this to a patient with runner’s knee, even though sensitization can be involved in any kind of chronic pain — the advice here is justified by the particularly important role that sensitization plays in low back pain, combined with the fact that low back pain is often highly sensitive to position/movement.

It’s certainly not justified by evidence! There is no direct evidence for the efficacy of this approach to the problem. It’s just pure plausibility and safety.

“But I’m always trying to spend time in the most comfortable position. How is this different?”

Fair question! SCS is more deliberate and methodical. We do naturally seek out positions that reduce our pain, but most of us can do a better job of it, more precise and thorough. It can be a “treatment” instead of reflexive pain avoidance, which is often:

  • too brief and erratic
  • distracted and agitated
  • only partial avoidance

For instance, if you are avoiding pain while still trying to work, you may adopt a position of greater comfort in your chair… but it’s still painful. That’s not a position of actual comfort, it’s a compromise, the least painful position that allows you to keep working.

It’s like the difference between falling asleep on the couch and meditating.

It’s also like the difference beween unconscious squirming and deliberate pain free range of motion exercises.

A back pain example

Let’s pretend to be a fly on the wall observing a man with back pain aggravated by postural stress at a desk. This fellow feels quite a bit better when he sits up a little straighter and actually uses the chair’s lumbar support (not an endorsement of lumbar support, just how this patient’s pain happens to work). Our subject can be seen doing this dozens of times per day, seeking relief, but not really finding it, because that position of greater comfort is never complete or sustained.

No matter how long we watch, all we see is reflexive and rather desperate and disorganized pain avoidance, which quickly succumbs to postural habit.

It’s also not completely comfortable. It’s less uncomfortable than his habitual slouch, but it’s not truly comfortable.

What really feels good to him is lying down with lumbar support. What if that person were to take a ten minute break and lay down, with lumbar support, relaxing in a position where the pain is truly the mildest, or even absent entirely?

And what if he was mentally calm and focused on that experience, rather than a barely conscious adjustment while preoccupied with stressful work? Is it possible that this patient might emerge from that more deliberate rest feeling far better?

You can bet your boots it is — hardly guaranteed of course, but “possible”? Sure.

What does it mean if it doesn’t work?

Failure doesn’t mean much. Although success is probably an indication that sensitization is a factor, with failure the interpretation could go either way:

  1. There’s still something wrong with your tissues.
  2. The false/excessive alarm is still false/excessive.

There’s no reason to think that genuine tissue distress will be helped much by positional release. If you have a small cyst pressing on a nerve root, or a slow-healing overuse injury to a intervertebral disc, the pain will probably resume as soon as the comfortable position is abandoned and the vulnerable tissue is disturbed.

But other kinds of low back pain may not resume hurting again — or not so fast — because there may not be much wrong with the tissue to disturb, or not so much as there was originally.

Level up: resting at the edge of the comfort zone

Another way to do positional release is to find the edge of a comfortable position and hang out there. Hanging out in the middle of your comfort zone is saying to your nervous system, “Hey, see? We’re just fine!” But we can move to the edge of the zone and add this: “We’re fine even over here where the alarm starts to pipe up!”

For instance, let’s say that you begin to experience back pain with a disconcertingly tiny degree of flexion: the pain starts at just 5˚ of unsupported stooping. So hang out at 4˚, probably with some support. Stand in front of a counter or table, give yourself some support to make sure your nervous system feels safe, add another degree, maybe another, until you can just start to hear the pain-alarm starting to blare… and pause there.

Just spend time there not having much of a problem.

Positional release as “exercise”

Rehab exercise can be divided into a hierarchy of intensity: pain-free range of motion, mobilizations, endurance, and strength training. Although neutral positioning barely registers as an “exercise,” it fits in nicely at the easy end of that spectrum.

Positional release can be considered the first stage in a rehab journey back to normal activity levels: just comfortable stillness instead of gentle painless movement, but both with the goal of gently reassuring the nervous system and “practicing” being pain free. Even as you progress with more challenging activity and exercise, you can also periodically interject a little positional release, to continue reassuring your central nervous system.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Don’t worry about lifting technique

A typical workplace safety sign cautioning against improper lifting technique, superimposed with bright read text, “Alarmist nonsense.”

The risks of poor lifting technique have been exaggerated.

The conventional wisdom is that we must not stoop to lift heavy objects. To avoid injury, we should squat down and then lift with our legs, not our backs. About 75% of physical therapists believe this,434 and the number is probably even higher outside that profession. It’s just as common for professionals to blame awkward and uneven lifting of lesser loads, as in this perfect example from a reader:

Every doctor and chiropractor and physiotherapist I have seen for months has told me that I have back pain because I’m carrying around my baby son. I don’t buy it! I had the same back pain for years before he was born. I don’t understand why they can’t understand that logic. It comes and it goes and I just don’t know why, but it isn’t my son, or it’s not just my son.

Her logic was solid: if the pain pre-dated motherhood, then baby-toting is a daft explanation for it.

All those pros who believe in the importance of good lifting technique are probably wrong. I will argue that this is worse than wrong — it’s actually counter-productive. The truth is undoubtedly in the middle, but decades ago the pendulum of public opinion and “common sense” swung all the way to one side and got stuck there; I think it needs a firm (evidence-based) push back towards the centre.

For a more thorough and technical review of both sides of this topic, see Greg Lehman’s excellent review, “Revisiting the spinal flexion debate: prepare for doubt.” This article focusses just on making the case that the conventional wisdom is a myth: stooping to lift is not a significant risk factor for back pain, and most people don’t need to be taught how to lift simple heavy objects “properly.” Several sub-topics are not covered here, especially the athletic extremes (like powerlifting), special occupational challenges (like nursing or piano moving), or lifting during rehab.

Your back isn’t fragile and you already know how to lift things

Although we can’t lift heavy things just any old way, we don’t really need to be taught either. What matters most is so obvious that it’s hard to get wrong: just keep objects close to the body.436 As long as we do that, there’s not much we can do to improve on it, and in particular neither stooping nor squatting has an obvious safety advantage (the science coming below).

Although some of this still sounds a bit contrarian and radical today, the value of trying to tinker with people’s lifting habits to has been under fire for a long time. In 1997, Dr. Nortin Hadler wrote a paper for the journal Spine with the subtitle: “what you lift or how you lift matters far less than whether you lift or when.”437 In 2002, physical therapist Leon Straker wrote:438

Little evidence supports the effectiveness of training programs to change workers’ lifting habits and any attempt at change may just increase risk as workers lose the protection of well practiced and conditioned movement patterns.

That opinion was backed by a 2008 review of several years’ worth of evidence about lifting technique and low back pain:439

There is no evidence to support use of advice or training in [lifting] techniques … for preventing back pain or consequent disability. The findings challenge current widespread practice of advising workers on correct lifting technique.

(I’ll review some more specific and recent evidence below.)

I think that training people to lift “properly” probably doesn’t work because backs are actually tough as good boots, and what makes backs hurt (or get injured) isn’t influenced all that much — if at all — by how you lift things. The conventional wisdom is based on an assumption of a fragility that just doesn’t exist in the back, so it’s not too surprising that the training doesn’t make much difference: there’s no vulnerability to avoid.

And that’s not the only bogus assumption in this mess.

Does heavy lifting actually increase the risk of back pain?

If lifting heavy things at work leads to back pain, then it would make more sense to be careful about how you do it. If.

As much as I appreciate their conclusions, Martimo et al. begin their paper with a whopper of another unjustified assumption, in the first sentence: “Heavy lifting at work increases the risk of back pain.”

If that assumption isn’t correct, the entire discussion is a moot point, right? And yet the authors support it with only a single reference to a 1999 paper published in an obscure journal, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics440 … and that paper supports nothing of the kind. It does not show that “heavy lifting increases the risk of back pain.”441 It’s a bogus citation! I am not making this up.

But that’s now ancient data in any case. Much more recently, a 2010 review concluded it’s “unlikely” that lifting was a cause of back pain in workers.442 A 2012 review found little to no evidence for any connection between back pain stooping over repeatedly or for long periods443 — a different angle on the same problem. Not enough reviews for you? Okay: a 2011 review of eight reviews “did not support” the conventional wisdom either.444

Never mind the weight: how about just the amount of time spent bending over? The stoopage factor? A 2015 study of 198 workers not only failed to find a link between the amount of back flexion and higher pain intensity, they found the opposite: more time spent flexing beyond 30˚ was linked to lower back pain intensity!445

None of this means that no one will ever hurt their back lifting something at work, but obviously the connection is nowhere near as obvious as everyone assumes. (Even the experts assumed it until quite recent history.) Likely there are major X factors.446

There’s no real smoke around lifting, so there’s probably no back pain fire.

Does stooping even put more load on the spine?

It is almost impossible not to flex your spine when lifting something off the ground, and there is remarkably little difference between spinal loading in different lifting techniques. Kingma et al measured 40˚ of spinal flexion even in a pure squat lift,447 the theoretical ideal lifting technique as understood by most people. Meanwhile, the lumbar spine flexed only ten degrees more when lifting the same way.

Or what if we could measure spinal forces directly? Imagine a pressure meter implanted in your back, completely replacing one of your vertebrae. What would it tell you, if you stooped over to pick up an object instead of squatting down to lift with your legs instead of your back? The nearly universal assumption is that stooping puts much more strain on the spine.

But if that was a safe assumption, I wouldn’t be bringing it up here. Just as Kingma et al found, there’s not much difference.

Those meters are actually a thing! Instrumented vertebral body replacements (VBRs) are high-tech gadgets installed in place of a vertebrae. Very cyborg! (Stronger, faster, more … measured?) In a 2016 experiment,448 three patients with VBRs did a bunch of lifting, and their implants measured the forces in squats versus stoops. Not only was the difference was negligible… it was actually in favour of stooping! Squatting is the supposedly “correct” and safe way to lift, but it actually caused 4% more load on tissues. Not a big number, but it’s in exactly the wrong direction if you were trying to support the conventional wisdom.

The current in vivo biomechanical study does not provide evidence that spinal loads differ substantially between stoop and squat lifting.

This is not perfect evidence, or the only evidence, but it’s enough to cast a lot of doubt on the value of advice to “lift with your legs, not your back.” And that’s all we need to make the case that the importance of lifting technique has been exaggerated.

What about back braces and support belts?

No one lifts more than bodybuilders and powerlifters. And bodybuilders must wear those big thick belts for some reason!449 If it makes sense for them, it must make sense for occupational lifting too. Surely.

Unless it doesn’t make sense for them. Siewe et al found that the use of weight belts increased the injury rate of the lumbar spine in powerlifters.450 Ruh roh!

And major recent reviews of the science have shown that there’s little or no prevention benefit to such belts in the workplace.451452 •sad trombone•

Interestingly, even hard braces are amazingly ineffective at reducing the forces on the spine!453 See Spinal Fracture Bracing and Fixation: My wife’s terrible accident, and a whirlwind tour of the science and biomechanics of her spine brace — fascinating topic.

Supports, braces, and belts mostly just provide some novel sensory input that reinforces the idea of security and stability — a sensation-aided placebo. That is, you don’t just hope that it supports your back, it feels like it does. Unfortunately, this also strongly encourages the insidious idea that backs need stabilizing in the first place. And that’s how you lose The Mind Game in Low Back Pain.

Another lesson from powerlifting

Deadlifts do not remotely look like a “safe” way to lift something heavy with your back. And yet the sport of powerlifting demonstrates that it’s possible to do deadlifts regularly without any obvious pattern of vulnerability to back pain. These guys and gals are stooping over and picking up dramatically more weight than anyone is ever going to lift at work.454 For fun. With, science says, less injury than other sports!455

Most powerlifters try to minimize spinal flexion, especially lumbar flexion, but it’s not clear that many of them are actually succeeding … and for sure many amateurs definitely fail, either because of poor training and/or because it is biomechanically difficult to achieve. Remember, it is nearly impossible not to flex your spine when lifting something off the ground.456 And so most deadlifts and strongman lifts bear a striking resemblance to how people are not supposed to lift, and yet the sport is amazingly safe.

But this isn’t about powerlifting: I’m just using powerlifting as an example to make a point about saner loads. I am definitely not saying that it’s safe for an untrained person to try to lift huge loads willy nilly — technique does matter when you’re trying to get several hundred pounds off the ground! It’s a completely different thing than schlepping stuff around in a warehouse. I am only saying that the range of what it’s possible to do surprisingly safely is just huge. If backs were actually prone to injuries when lifting 20-40 kilos with poor technique or training, it’s unlikely that people could ever safely multiply that by 3-10 times in deadlifts, but they clearly do, even with imperfect elimination of flexion, if that is even possible.

The point is that backs are naturally sturdy and non-fragile, and powerlifting is a great demonstration of that.

Don’t worry about how you lift … but don’t be a fool either!

Obviously you can hurt yourself if you are reckless with heavy loads. And obviously technique does matter for extreme loads (the kind of loads no one would ever be expected to deal with at work). Strain hard enough and you will get a muscle strain (a tear), or worse. And although disc herniations may be less common and less serious and less related to either lifting or back pain than people think, that doesn’t mean you want one.

But training for lifting technique is probably not important because heavy lifting itself probably does not actually increase the risk of back pain significantly in the first place — and so there’s no problem to solve with better technique, and no evidence that there is even any way to significantly improve on our technique. Doubtless heavy lifting is at least a little bit of a factor in back pain, just not a major one — not the kind of factor that generates a nice clear statistical signal.

Back pain that starts with a lifting trauma probably occurs less than most people think, and isn’t as severe, and when it does occur it probably often seems worse than it is due to the common problem of trigger points.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Traction: low back pain on the rack!

Traction sure sounds like a good idea! Most people with low back pain feel as though their vertebrae are jammed together, and crave relief from that sensation. Gravity relentlessly presses vertebrae together — wouldn’t it feel nice to go the other way?

And it does. Nearly everyone seems to appreciate the feeling of lightening the load. But does it actually solve anything? Or is it just a temporary relief, an itch scratching? It may feel “as if” vertebrae are jammed together, but is that really the problem? In a handful of cases, it’s possible that traction is genuinely biomechanically relevant and modestly therapeutic. But even at best it’s probably analogous to getting off your feet when you have a rock in your shoe, without actually removing the rock — that is, a brief reduction of loading on sensitive tissue, but you don’t actually solve anything.

The science of traction is overwhelmingly negative, decent evidence that traction is effective for the average back pain patient. Like an aspirin that works only 5% of the time, it’s not a very useful treatment. And yet people crave it, and will do it, and maybe a 5% chance isn’t so bad when you’re desperate.

Just don’t pay too much for it. That’s the key.

There are a lot of ways to pull on your back, ranging from the cheap and easy to expensive and elaborate and pretty obviously scammy. Because traction often feels good, and because it might help, it’s probably worth cautiously experimenting with the cheaper and safer options.

But it’s probably not worth experimenting with the more expensive and risky options, such as “non-surgical spinal decompression therapy” (SDT) machines — the rack. Mechanical traction pulls harder and longer on your back, draining your bank account more and increasing the risks, all without any evidence of benefit.

These machines are well-studied, and most of the evidence about the effectiveness of back traction in general comes from studies of SDT. For the purposes of debunking, I report on that evidence in detail in a separate section devoted to criticizing SDT machines and claims. The bottom line is clear, though, hammered home in a major review of the subject in 2007 that looked at all the science up to then: “ … traction as a single treatment for LBP is probably not effective.”457 That certainly doesn’t exclude all hope — what about multiple treatments? — but it is discouraging.

For the remainder of this section, I will focus on the cheaper, simpler, and safer methods…

Seated lumbar pulls. The simplest method of low back traction is incredibly easy and free — just sit somewhere where your knees can hook firmly over the edge (i.e. a firm bed), lean back a little, place your hands flat beside your hips, and push backwards. You’re simply using your arm strength to push your upper body away from your hips, using the edge as an anchor. Hand position matters — you may have to tinker a bit, placing them farther forward or back. But if you have the principle in mind, you should be able to easily apply a gentle stretch.

Therapist-powered traction is another good option to consider. Although a therapist’s time is not inexpensive, traction can be just one inexpensive (fairly brief) component of the session. Therapists apply traction primarily by pulling on legs or hips, sometimes with the assistance of straps and other simple tools. One of the virtues of therapist-powered traction is that it there is only so much force that can be applied, making this a safe way to possibly relieve some discomfort. If it seems helpful, you can also graduate to an inversion table for a stronger traction.

Inversion tables are simple machines that dangle you upside down. They are a reasonably good compromise approach to traction, providing a middlin’ degree of traction at a middlin’ cost with middlin’ safety. Although they still require an investment of around $200 and some floor space, the expense is one-time only, and the equipment can be returned or sold if it doesn’t work for you. The cost is worth it if it works, and trivial if it doesn’t.

An inversion table

The inversion table allows good control over the intensity of traction as you slowly tilt yourself upside down.

There are many different models, but I emphatically recommend the table design of inversion machinery, in which you lie on a tilting platform that allows you to gently and slowly lower yourself into an increasingly inverted position. Being supported and having control are important when you’re experimenting with a treatment that could cause problems. By contrast, there are inversion machines which involve simply hanging loose from boots. These might be appropriate for someone who’s already established that they like to hang upside down … but what a bad way to figure it out!

Spinal “jostlers” like the Back2Life tool. The idea is that you lie on your back, hook your knees over supports that slightly lift your butt off the ground, and then the machine oscillates gently, creating “continuous passive motion” — wiggling you while you relax. I certainly don’t care for the style of the marketing for the product, and I definitely don’t think it “aligns the spine” or really anything at all that the website claims. But I do like the sensations produced by the device, and the combination of a little mobilizing and a little traction. It’s a pleasing, interesting source of stimulation, and I think that is often useful for back pain. It’s also mild enough to be extremely safe, and it’s not super expensive (about $200 USD).

Inflatable belts, like Dr. Ho’s Decompression Belt. “As the belt inflates with air, it expands vertically, gently stretching and tractioning the lower back.” But can it actually push apart spinal joints? It’s a little doubtful. I discuss the mechanics of bracing in another section.

Why doesn’t traction work better?

How come traction doesn’t pass scientific tests with flying colours? Decompression of intervertebral joints and discs is unlikely to help unless “compression” is the problem. And, even if compression was the problem, decompression can only temporarily and slightly relieve it. You’re going to have to stand up again soon.

However, many lines of evidence show that compressed and pinched structures are probably much less likely to be the explanation for back pain than most people think. By far the most common alleged compression in low back pain is the pinching of nerve roots — which this tutorial has already thoroughly debunked as an important factor of low back pain.

Another candidate for cranky compressed anatomy is the facet joints. However, facet joints have also been shown to be a factor only in some cases of low back pain, and they are not significantly load bearing. They do bear load — just nowhere near as much as the intervertebral discs. Thus temporarily relieving the back from vertical loading can only do so much for the facet joints.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Stretching as a back pain treatment

Stretching is over-rated: it enjoys a reputation as a pillar of fitness and a staple of rehab that it does not deserve.458 And people certainly believe stretching helps back pain, in large numbers.459

Stretching may have some modest value for some low back pain patients, but the benefits are probably erratic and minor, and in general you’ll get better bang for buck just from doing exercise and physical activities you enjoy, or anything that’s just more active — like mobilization exercises I’ve already recommended.

Nevertheless, I do think it’s worth dabbling in stretching. The stakes are high with back pain, after all. And it’s easy to experiment safely with stretching. So it’s probably worth some experimenting.

The science of stretching for back pain is some of the worst in all of musculoskeletal medicine

Stretching for back pain is both under studied and badly studied — like everything else in this business only more so. Despite widespread public faith in stretching as back pain medicine, making it a fine topic to focus on, unfortunately researchers usually fail to focus on it.460 So don’t expect much data-driven enlightenment here, but I’ll do my best. The literature is littered with studies that seem “promising,” without ever being decisive. Most of these seemingly positive results evaporate like water on a hot sidewalk when examined more closely. Here’s three telling examples:

  1. A 2010 experiment described in the Archives of Internal Medicine comparing yoga, a stretching class, and an educational booklet.461 This study has been widely reported as “stretching and yoga work,” and it is indeed a good-news trial on its face. What has not been reported is that both stretching and yoga are equally damned here with faint praise: the effect size was modest, just 2.5 points on a scale of 11, which is just barely clinically meaningful. And even that modest benefit could well be an artifact.462 Not only is it not persuasive, I think it backfires and convinces me that yoga/stretching really didn’t do much.
  2. In 2014, Chen et al reported “significant” benefit for nurses with back pain who stretched three times per week after work for six months.463 The results are right in line with what people optimistically expect of a stretching habit: that it’s at least as good as popping ibuprofen. Isn’t it great when Science tells us what we already believe? Unfortunately, it’s a terrible study, just complete garbage, in several ways.464 I only cited this paper so I could point at it and laugh before pivoting to the disappointing reality.
  3. In 2019, Pourahmadi et al reviewed 12 studies and reported happy news about “slump stretching” for back pain465 — which looks like seated toe-touching, but with a couple extra twists to really crank up tension on the spinal cord, like tucking the chin. This is a fine example of a not-so-positive-after-all test in two key ways. First, it’s not actually a muscle stretch: its explicit target is “neuromeningeal structures within the vertebral canal” (neurodynamic stretching) and so even if the study was believably positive, it’s not validating anything that most people would ever intentionally do for their back pain.466 Second, even if it wasn’t obscure, the quality of the evidence they reviewed “was very low.” There’s never any justification for writing a positive-sounding result based on limited, lousy evidence. For all I know, slump stretching might truly work, and could be a ray of hope that some back pain can be treated with stretch (presumably the kind of back pain that involves the spinal cord, so definitely not all), but we cannot know that based on a few scraps of shabby data.

I am unaware of any study of this question that is actually promising. Meanwhile, there is plenty of actual “evidence of absence.” Three examples:

  • In 2014, Sihawong et al showed in a roundabout way that stretching doesn’t prevent back pain, even in people with poor trunk flexibility.467 They studied hundreds of office workers with poor flexibility and trunk muscle endurance, but no back pain (yet), and followed them for a year while half of did exercises and half didn’t. That’s a fine design in many ways, so it’s a shame that they didn’t isolate their variables: like so many other studies, they combined stretching with other kinds of exercise. Unsurprisingly, the exercise group got less than half as many new cases of back pain. We already know that exercise is helpful … and that almost certainly accounts for the entire risk reduction. Not the stretching.
  • An interesting 2021 study compared stretching unfavourably to a more elaborate exercise therapy, “motor skills training.”468 Stretching lost (and so did strength training). Not by a lot, but it didn’t have to. The point is that stretching failed to outperform some other exercise approach most people have never even heard of — which wasn’t impressive either. Even with unimpressive competition, stretching was not a contender.
  • A 2020 review of studies of exercise for back pain — a good quality one in British Journal of Sports Medicine — concluded that stretching doesn’t work.469

That’s enough science of general stretching for back pain. Let’s get more specific now.

Could some of the apparent benefits of stretching be attributable to helping trigger points? And could we get more benefit from stretching with a more specific focus on muscle imbalance? The next two chapters will cover these ideas.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Stretching out those mini-cramps?

This chapter focusses on one of the main reasons to consider stretching despite the underwhelming evidence presented so far: as a treatment for muscle soreness specifically, as a way to loose the muscle “knots.” It’s a summary of a much longer exploration of the topic in the trigger point tutorial, which is why this summary deliberately lacks a lot of footnotes and references. (Note also that this topic has already been explored in a different way in the yoga chapter.)

I have rarely even heard anecdotes about stretching actually “curing” back pain — major, lasting relief just doesn’t seem to be a level of potency that gets claimed very often. But many, many people report a “takes the edge off” effect. So people in all kinds of pain tend to stretch, and often they feel better, briefly. To the extent that this is true, it might be because stretching can sometimes partially treat trigger points. It’s an interesting idea to explore. In particular, if it works at all, I think it’s interesting to ask why it isn’t more effective.

Simons and Mense write (in their text, Muscle Pain), that stretching “by almost any means is beneficial” for trigger points — a ringing endorsement from the world’s leading authorities on trigger point pain. That sure sounds good!

But they also emphasize that (1) it has “not been firmly established” that stretching trigger points is helpful, and that (2) it works primarily for “newly activated, single-muscle” trigger points (which leaves out a lot of trigger points that are serious problems), and they also caution that (3) the stretch must be applied “slowly and only to the onset of discomfort” … which is a complete contradiction to their supposition that a trigger point must be “fully elongated” to deactivate it!

Folks, there is no hope of “fully elongating” most muscles by applying stretch “slowly and only to the onset of discomfort.” People with truly nasty trigger points can’t fully elongate a Slinky without an onset of major discomfort, let alone their trigger point-riddled muscles. Trigger points are tough — they are a tiny patch of spasmed muscle tissue, after all. And the muscle on either side of them is relatively elastic and already under strain from the trigger point. This could present a real obstacle to stretching serious trigger points. How do you elongate a patch of muscle that’s much harder and more tightly contracted than the tissue around it? Indeed, some trigger points are clearly aggravated by stretching!

So, basically, stretching is imprecise and inefficient! Trigger points are small and numerous and tough as nails, while stretches are big and imprecise and slow and awkward. And stretches may not be capable of elongating trigger points even when the stretch is strong and easily sustained.

Stretching probably does have at least a small therapeutic effect on milder trigger points some of the time. And this probably explains why it often feels so good and partially, temporarily relieves pain and stiffness. But that modest benefit falls way short of the goals I have for my patients — and it falls short of your goals, too, if you have serious low back pain.

So, feel free to experiment, but beware of wasting your time! For most people, there are other treatment methods that are almost certainly more useful!

Once again, see the trigger points tutorial for the full discussion of stretching for trigger points, which is about five times longer than this section and gets into all the nitty gritty detail.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Further Reading

Other articles on PainScience.com about back pain (lots of them, as if this book wasn’t long enough):

Other good reading about back pain:

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Reader feedback … good and bad

Testimonials on health care websites reek of quackery, so publishing them has always made me a bit queasy. But my testimonials are mostly about the quality of the information I’m selling, and I hope that makes all the difference. So here’s some highlights from the kind words I’ve received over the years … plus some of the common criticisms I receive, at the end. These are all genuine testimonials, mostly received by email. In many cases I withold or change names and identifying details.

As an author and a chronic lower back pain sufferer, after spending the day with your book I am supremely impressed. You present such useful information in such an entertaining fashion. I’ve been laughing and learning and feeling completely uplifted and delighted and confident that I’ll be able to sort out my back pain issues with your book as a guide.

Anna Herald


I was reading your info about lower right back pain and I love how you say not to worry about everything, and add that even if it IS serious, it can most likely be cured. While reading it, I realised later that my acute pain had slightly subsided.You’re doing such a great service for so many people. I’m almost glad I was in such pain (on a weekend, of course) that I had to look up medical information myself. Yours is readable, understandable, funny, humble, and....tight on!

Elsie Ventura


Just wanted to thank you for your clear, well-organized, credible, and thorough information about back pain. I am experiencing back pain that’s barking loudly for the first time in my 72 years. I now have an idea of the possibilities and degrees of seriousness. Thank you again for making the time and effort to write well.

Braydon Aldred


I stumbled upon your writings this morning, when I was wishing I was at the gym, where I would usually be. It was some kind of kismet, for sure, because everything I read made total, absolute sense. Thank you, thank you, thank you. You have given me the courage and resolve to sit out this sprain so I can get back up again intact, and stay that way. Your writings about joint mobilization also make me realize I have some tactics to improve my current state now, even though I am recovering. A huge lightbulb has gone off for me. Thank you, again! Finally, I love your statement about going and putting your 110% into something else while you are healing. Best advice ever.

Stacey Simons


Thanks again for putting together all the back pain research — a good percentage of it anyway — into an easy-to-read publication.

Nojus Proctor


Thank you immensely for the best, most sensible and credible information on back pain. So glad you came up in my google search. Credible, reassuring, and humorous too.

Suzanne Haslett, Suzanne Haslett, Masters in Human Development (Oxford)


I purchased the low back tutorial recently and got the free trigger points one also. Many thanks. They are great! I had already accessed the perfect spot series and have been working on my trigger points. It is very pleasing to have the full discussion of the physiology, and I now have a much better idea about the whole ghastly business.

Leah Brannen, Saskatoon, Canada


I bought two of your eBooks last week, and I’m enjoying going through them. Your presentation is excellent. It’s far too early to say, of course, but I think I’ve already begun to benefit from your approach. One of the things I like most about your approach is your respect for “science,” as opposed to “merchandising.” You've put so much into those two eBooks, it's going to take time to do them the justice they deserve.

David Calderisi, Toronto, Ontario

David diligently followed up a month later with the following comment: “By now I’m convinced your research and recommendations are right on the money. Thanks. I’ve recommended you to a few people who, like myself, have had back problems on and off for years. Thanks for having provided such a useful tool.” ~ Paul

I spent seven hours straight sitting at my computer and my back didn't hurt at all! You’re a genius! Thanks so much.

Isabelle Deguise, Graduate Student


I had suffered from undiagnosed and seemingly untreatable low back pain since late August last year. Three physiotherapists, my GP, two RMTs, and my generally excellent personal trainer failed to help me make any progress. At my last visit to my GP in late December, he maintained his insistence that I just needed to loosen up my hamstrings! The systematic approach you took to reviewing all the supposed cures and providing a clear analysis of each and no doubt saved me thousands of dollars and months of frustration. That gave me the focus to work on trigger points known to cause LBP (with the help of some additional books and a great TP therapy app for my phone). My back pain isn’t totally gone, but I’m 95% there and I’ve got a handle on it.

Erin Banks


One more noteworthy endorsement, with regards to this whole website and all of my books, submitted by a London physician specializing in chronic pain, medical education, and patient-advocacy (that’s a link to his excellent blog):

I’m writing to congratulate and thank you for your impressive ongoing review of musculoskeletal research. I teach a course, Medicine in Society, at St. Leonards Hospital in Hoxton. I originally stumbled across your website whilst looking for information about pain for my medical students, and have recommended your tutorials to them. Your work deserves special mention for its transparency, evidence base, clear presentation, educational content, regular documented updates, and lack of any commercial promotional material.

Dr. Jonathon Tomlinson, MBBS, DRCOG, MRCGP, MA, The Lawson Practice, London

What about criticism and complaints?

Oh, I get those too! I do not host public comments on PainScience.com for many reasons, but emailed constructive criticism, factual corrections, requests, and suggestions are all very welcome. I have made many important changes to this tutorial inspired directly by critical, informed reader feedback.

But you can’t make everyone happy! Some people demand their money back (and get it). I have about a 1% refund rate (far better than average in retail/e-commerce). The complaints of my most dissatisfied customers have strong themes:

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Acknowledgements

Thanks to every reader, client, and book customer for your curiosity, your faith, and your feedback and suggestions, and your stories most of all — without you, all of this would be impossible and pointless.

Writers go on and on about how grateful they are for the support they had while writing one measly book, but this website is actually a much bigger project than a book. PainScience.com was originally created in my so-called “spare time” with a lot of assistance from family and friends (see the origin story). Thanks to my wife for countless indulgences large and small; to my parents for (possibly blind) faith in me, and much copyediting; and to friends and technical mentors Mike, Dirk, Aaron, and Erin for endless useful chats, repeatedly saving my ass, plus actually building many of the nifty features of this website.

Special thanks to some professionals and experts who have been particularly inspiring and/or directly supportive: Dr. Rob Tarzwell, Dr. Steven Novella, Dr. David Gorski, Sam Homola, DC, Dr. Mark Crislip, Scott Gavura, Dr. Harriet Hall, Dr. Stephen Barrett, Dr. Greg Lehman, Dr. Jason Silvernail, Todd Hargrove, Nick Ng, Alice Sanvito, Dr. Chris Moyer, Lars Avemarie, PT, Dr. Brian James, Bodhi Haraldsson, Diane Jacobs, Adam Meakins, Sol Orwell, Laura Allen, James Fell, Dr. Ravensara Travillian, Dr. Neil O’Connell, Dr. Tony Ingram, Dr. Jim Eubanks, Kira Stoops, Dr. Bronnie Thompson, Dr. James Coyne, Alex Hutchinson, Dr. David Colquhoun, Bas Asselbergs … and almost certainly a dozen more I am embarrassed to have neglected.

I work “alone,” but not really, thanks to all these people.

I have some relationship with everyone named above, but there are also many experts who have influenced me that I am not privileged to know personally. Some of the most notable are: Drs. Lorimer Moseley, David Butler, Gordon Waddell, Robert Sapolsky, Brad Schoenfeld, Edzard Ernst, Jan Dommerholt, Simon Singh, Ben Goldacre, Atul Gawande, and Nikolai Boguduk.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

What’s new in this tutorial?

This tutorial has been continuously, actively maintained and updated for 19 years now, staying consistent with professional guidelines and the best available science. The first edition was originally published in September 2004, after countless hours of research and writing while I spent a month taking care of a farm (and a beautiful pair of young puppies) in the Okanagan.

Regular updates are a key feature of PainScience.com tutorials. As new science and information becomes available, I upgrade them, and the most recent version is always automatically available to customers. Unlike regular books, and even e-books (which can be obsolete by the time they are published, and can go years between editions) this document is updated at least once every three months and often much more. I also log updates, making it easy for readers to see what’s changed. This tutorial has gotten 237 major and minor updates since I started logging carefully in late 2009 (plus countless minor tweaks and touch-ups).

Sep 21, 2023 — Rewritten: Like-new re-write. Before: simplistic and old-school by my 2023 standards. After: not very scientifically rigorous, but much more informed and wide-ranging speculation. And kinda fun. [Updated section: Could low back pain be an overuse injury?]

August — New chapter: No notes. Just a new chapter. [Updated section: Self-treatment of slipped rib syndrome.]

August — New chapter: Not just a new chapter, but a particularly huge one, based on interesting new science, with plenty of value. (PainSci Members can also read this in a recent blog post, “Extra, extra! Extra floating ribs are way more common than anyone knew.”) [Updated section: Ribs and back pain.]

August — New chapter: No notes. Just a new chapter. [Updated section: Caring for back pain the ‘biopsychosocial’ way.]

June — Science update: Cited Schmidt on lordosis assessment reliability (there is none). [Updated section: Are you crooked? The alignment and posture villains: short legs, pelvic tilts, and spinal curves.]

May — Edited: Numerous minor corrections and improvements. Added ‘focalization’ as a clearer alternative to ‘centralization.’ Added two images. There's also now a blog post version of this chapter for PainSci members, and an audio version of the chapter embedded here. [Updated section: Centralization, directional preference, and (mostly) extension exercises.]

May — Small additions: Post-publication editing and polishing, most notably a couple more citations about MDT efficacy, and an interesting point about the implications of centralization’s origin story. [Updated section: The McKenzie Method® of Mechanical Diagnosis and Therapy® (MDT).]

May — New chapter: No notes. Just a new chapter. [Updated section: The McKenzie Method® of Mechanical Diagnosis and Therapy® (MDT).]

May — New chapter: No notes. Just a new chapter. [Updated section: Centralization, directional preference, and (mostly) extension exercises.]

April — Rewritten and expanded: This chapter was completely overhauled and greatly increased in size. Although still primarily based on a 2009 study, it’s not a much more general chapter about back pain prognosis and chronicity. There’s more and recent detailed follow-up evidence. There are some new graphs and imagery. There’s much more useful information about risk factors. And tThe previous reassurance-is-good-for-back-pain slant has been replaced with a much more nuanced perspective on the relationship between back pain and psychological factors. [Updated section: Chronic low back pain is not so chronic: the pseudo-myth of chronicity.]

February — Revised: After several years without much attention, this chapter has finally gotten a variety of improvements. [Updated section: Diagnose, schmiagnose! Structural problems in the low back are (very) hard to diagnose accurately.]

2022 — Upgraded: Added more about the role of general exercise in treating back pain. [Updated section: Massage with movement and life in the Goldilocks zone — light and general exercise.]

2022 — Upgraded: Revised and expanded to include a discussion of Hydrocollators. [Updated section: Heat and ice both provide good bang for buck, but err on the side of heat.]

2022 — Rewritten: Completely rebooted, all new everything about scoliosis treatment. [Updated section: Scoliosis cannot be straightened.]

2022 — Rewritten: Some of the original chapter remains, but mostly I just started over. [Updated section: The strange case of scoliosis.]

2022 — Minor revision: Polished and tweaked. Nothing fancy, just good old fashioned editing. [Updated section: It’s not structure, except when it is: “specific” back pain.]

2022 — Major revision: Major revisions and improvements. Over the years, this chapter got a bit muddled and obsolete, just an old rant I hadn't properly updated in over a decade. I had a bunch of fresh yoga science in my head after writing the new yoga chapter, and so this was a perfect time to reboot the chapter. [Updated section: Stress relief and the tyranny of meditation and yoga.]

2022 — Additions: Restored some selected passages lost in the rewrite (and it’s now one of the longest chapters in the book, maybe the very longest, and longer than most standalone articles on the topic). Turning into a book within a book! [Updated section: Yoga has no “active ingredient” for back pain (but it’s still good exercise).]

2022 — Rewritten: A complete reboot of my coverage of the topic of yoga for back pain, with much more (and more recent) science. [Updated section: Yoga has no “active ingredient” for back pain (but it’s still good exercise).]

2022 — Science update: Added a much more detailed explanation of how the sensitivity of muscle has been demonstrated experimentally, based on Graven-Nielsen et al. [Updated section: There is nothing “just” about muscle.]

Archived updates — All updates, including 180 older updates, are listed on another page.

2005 — Publication.

GO TO TOPCONTENTSNOTES

Notes

  1. In 2010, the Journal of Bone & Joint Surgery reported that “the quality and content of health information on the internet is highly variable for common sports medicine topics” — a bit of an understatement, really. Expert reviewers examined about 75 top-ranked commercial websites and another 30 academic sites. They gave each a quality score on a scale of 100. The average score? Barely over 50! For more detail, see Starman et al. This reference is getting old, but nothing has really changed. 😜
  2. Machado LAC, Kamper SJ, Herbert RD, Maher CG, McAuley JH. Analgesic effects of treatments for non-specific low back pain: a meta-analysis of placebo-controlled randomized trials. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2009 May;48(5):520–7. PubMed 19109315 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54670 ❐

    This is the best single reference showing the lack of efficacy from all popular back pain treatments, but there’s lot more to say — much more information about treatment efficacy will be presented later on in this tutorial.

  3. Hexenschuss is a German word for back spasm or lumbago, but translated literally it means shot by the witch (hexe = witch and Schuss = shot). Those Germans have a word for everything! Hat tip to reader Richard Moison for teaching it to me.
  4. Here’s the first of many expert opinions and quotes about this, from a 2013 paper by Max Zusman, explaining why low back pain is still such an incredibly costly problem for society:

    It is extremely difficult to alter the potentially disabling belief among the lay public that low back pain has a structural mechanical cause. An important reason for this is that this belief continues to be regularly reinforced by the conditions of care of a range of ‘hands-on’ providers, for whom idiosyncratic variations of that view are fundamental to their professional existence.”

    Well said, but perhaps a bit wordy. Here’s the simple version: patients believe back pain is caused by structural fragility, and careers are built on catering to that belief. I would also say that it is difficult to alter that belief in anyone, patient or professional. This preoccupation with fragility isn’t just reinforced by the practices of many therapists, it’s a major reason for them.

  5. Jansson C, Mittendorfer-Rutz E, Alexanderson K. Sickness absence because of musculoskeletal diagnoses and risk of all-cause and cause-specific mortality: A nationwide Swedish cohort study. Pain. 2012 May;153(5):998–1005. PubMed 22421427 ❐
  6. Williams CM, Maher CG, Hancock MJ, et al. Low back pain and best practice care: a survey of general practice physicians. Arch Intern Med. 2010 Feb;170(3):271–7. PubMed 20142573 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55582 ❐

    “Usual care provided by general practitioners for low back pain does not match the care endorsed in international evidence-based guidelines and may not provide the best outcomes for patients. This situation has not improved over time.

  7. A series of 2018 back pain articles in The Lancet (see Foster, Hartvigsen, Buchbinder) expressed what has become almost standard outrage at the useless back pain treatments still constantly provided by all kinds of professionals. This has been going on for decades now: the experts bemoaning the pitiful state of the standard of care for back pain. The experts just keep saying it, seemingly with more force every year — more presitigous journals, citing better evidence than ever — while the average clinician remains largely oblivious.
  8. Doctors lack the skills and knowledge needed to care for most common aches, pains, and injury problems, especially the chronic cases, and even the best are poor substitutes for physical therapists. This has been proven in a number of studies, like Stockard et al, who found that 82% of medical graduates “failed to demonstrate basic competency in musculoskeletal medicine.” It’s just not their thing, and people with joint or meaty body pain should take their family doctor’s advice with a grain of salt. See The Medical Blind Spot for Aches, Pains & Injuries: Most physicians are unqualified to care for many common pain and injury problems, especially the more stubborn and tricky ones.
    Cartoon of a man sitting in a doctor’s office. The doctor is holding a clipboard with a checklist with just two items on it: stress related and age related. The caption reads: “An extremely general practitioner.”
  9. Buchbinder R, Staples M, Jolley D. Doctors with a special interest in back pain have poorer knowledge about how to treat back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009 May;34(11):1218–26; discussion 1227. PubMed 19407674 ❐
  10. I was an alternative health professional myself for many years — a Registered Massage Therapist, trained in Canada (which has unusually good training standards). Of course, some of my colleagues in alternative medicine were diligent students of medical science. However, in my experience, most were certainly not — indeed, many lacked even the most basic knowledge of how medical science works or how to keep current about recent discoveries with clinical implications.
  11. Moseley GL. Whole of community pain education for back pain. Why does first-line care get almost no attention and what exactly are we waiting for? Br J Sports Med. 2018 Jul. PubMed 29982226 ❐
  12. Moseley 2018, op. cit. Dr. Moseley again. He notes that “education is universally recommended as first-line treatment for acute and persistent back pain but it attracts little attention … Is this because health professionals intuitively know what education is and how to do it? Our research suggests not6—most doctors (although there are precious exceptions) do not know what it is, do not know how to do it, do not have the content knowledge and, even if they did, do not have the time.”
  13. Patients new to pain often assume that healthcare pros must know what it’s like… but most can’t actually relate, not without living with it. And so obnoxious overconfidence is common. Exhibit A: When internet-famous physical therapist Adam Meakins had an episode of severe back pain, he received endless bizarrely arrogant, judgemental, condescending reactions from his colleages! Adam Meakins:

    “Out of all the debates and debacles I’ve had on social media I don’t think I’ve ever been accused of, or called so many negative and nasty things by so many clinicians since I’ve been a person in pain documenting my progress over these last 9 days! #GiveMeStrength 🙄🤦🏻‍♂️🤫”

    I have seen this nonsense myself with regards to my own chronic pain. The subtext (or just the text!) is always the same: “If you were competent like me, you could prevent/solve your own painful problem.” Ironically, the truth is almost certainly the opposite: their arrogance clearly results in ignorance and incompetence. Most of them are doomed to be humbled by their own pain someday.

  14. [Cover of Mind Over Back Pain, by Dr. John Sarno]

    Dr. John Sarno’s original best-seller about low back pain.

    My original inspiration for this tutorial was Dr. John Sarno’s 1984 book Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain, which is generally excellent, but a little too radical, too excited about one big idea. His more recent work (Healing Back Pain: The mind-body connection) makes too many empty promises and has serious flaws. See my Sarno review.
  15. Why is musculoskeletal medicine such a mess? Many reasons, but mainly professional pride and tribalism, ideological momentum, and screwed up incentives (certification rackets, freelancing, insurance, huge profits). Also, the love of “advanced” and technological treatment methods, ignorance of the history of science and critical thinking skills, and the obscurity of newer and better ideas, especially the major neurological and biological factors that we’re still learning about, which many frontline clinicians are still oblivious to. I explore the trouble with modern musculoskeletal medicine in more detail in A Historical Perspective On Aches ‘n’ Pains: We are living in a golden age of pain science and musculoskeletal medicine … sorta.
  16. There are many relevant sources, but these recommended beliefs are all cribbed directly from a short but sweet 2020 paper in the British Journal of Sports Medicine, “Back to basics: 10 facts every person should know about back pain”. It’s a good, readable explanation of back pain myths, and includes a fine “back facts” infographic. Here are the “unhelpful” low back pain beliefs identified by O’Sullivan et al., all “culturally endorsed and not supported by evidence”:
    1. Low back pain is usually a serious medical condition.
    2. Low back pain will become persistent and deteriorate in later life.
    3. Persistent low back pain is always related to tissue damage.
    4. Scans are always needed to detect the cause of low back pain.
    5. Pain related to exercise and movement is always a warning that harm is being done to the spine and a signal to stop or modify activity.
    6. Low back pain is caused by weak “core” muscles and having a strong core protects against future Low back pain.
    7. Repeated spinal loading results in “wear and tear” and tissue damage.
    8. Pain flare-ups are a sign of tissue damage and require rest.
    9. Treatments such as strong medications, injectionss and surgery are effective, and necessary, to treat Low back pain.

    Good list! But I decided that a list of “helpful” beliefs would be more, well, helpful than a list of unhelpful ones, and editorially inverted them. 😉

  17. Coste J, Delecoeuillerie G, Cohen de Lara A, Le Parc JM, Paolaggi JB. Clinical course and prognostic factors in acute low back pain: an inception cohort study in primary care practice. BMJ. 1994;308:577–80. PubMed 8148683 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 57081 ❐

    This paper presents some unusually optimistic old data about low back pain recovery: in a sample of about 100 patients, “90% of patients recovered within two weeks and only two developed chronic low back pain,” which is “much higher than reported in other studies,” but the authors suggest some good reasons why their number makes sense.

  18. There is an anxiety-producing myth that low back pain is serious and chronic if you can’t shake it after the first several weeks, when in fact many people recover just fine after that “deadline.” An entire section below is devoted to the strong scientific evidence about this.
  19. Once in a great while some cranky reader (always a guy) writes to tell me, “I didn’t learn anything from your book.” I’m a little skeptical about that, and it’s always tempting to start quizzing! There’s a great deal of information here, including analyses of recent research. Sure, readers who have already done a lot of reading about back pain might already be familiar with a lot of it — but you will know that going in, of course, and you’ll find the nuggets of new information and perspective that any keen reader is always looking for.
  20. In older adults, about 6% of cases have a serious cause, but 5% of those are fractures — which are serious, but they aren’t cancer either. The 1% is divided amongst all other serious causes. For people under age 55, the odds are even better.
  21. This is my translation/interpretation for patients of the somewhat more detailed information for physicians published in “Diagnosis and Treatment of Low Back Pain: A Joint Clinical Practice Guideline from the American College of Physicians and the American Pain Society”.
  22. This is the best red flag we have for cauda equina syndrome (trouble with the lower spinal cord) — better than the more conventional . For more detail, see later in the book or When to Worry About Low Back Pain. But the main citation for this is Angus 2021.
  23. All systemic infections can cause body aches, fatigue, and fever, because these symptoms are functions of the immune system, not the disease — but some diseases, like COVID-19, provoke it more than others.

    The symptoms of most infections are not directly caused by damage they do to our tissues, especially at first. We cannot feel cells being killed by a virus; what we actually do feel is our immune system’s reaction to the invasion. One purpose of that reaction is to force us to stay still — also known as rest — mostly by making movement feel incredibly difficult and unpleasant. This “sickness behaviour” is a generalized reaction to a wide variety of biological threats found in all animals (see subtle systemic inflammation).

  24. Lei S, Jiang F, Su W, et al. Clinical characteristics and outcomes of patients undergoing surgeries during the incubation period of COVID-19 infection. EClinicalMedicine. 2020 2020/04/06. PainSci Bibliography 52605 ❐
  25. Report of the WHO-China Joint Mission on Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19). Accessed 2020-04-06.
  26. Ruhli FJ, Henneberg M. Clinical perspectives on secular trends of intervertebral foramen diameters in an industrialized European society. Eur J Spine. 2004 Apr 1. PubMed 15057552 ❐

    This study of changes in spinal characteristics (in Swiss adults) since the late 19th century found … well, basically nothing. Spines are pretty much the same now as they were then. At least in Switzerland.

  27. Dr. Richard Deyo, one of the great myth busters of low back pain research, believes that “low back pain is second to upper respiratory problems as a symptom-related reason for visits to a physician” — only the common cold causes more complaints. Hart et al puts low back pain in fifth place (lower because Hart oddly excludes chronic low back pain). Chronic low back pain is usually the kind that this book will examine. Andersson writes: “Although the literature is filled with information about the prevalence and incidence of back pain in general, there is less information about chronic back pain … .” Indeed, it is almost impossible to measure how much chronic low back pain there is: for every time that acute low back pain is the main reason for a visit to a physician, how many times does a patient mention low back pain as a secondary problem? Or sees an alternative health care professional about it instead? (Answer: pretty danged often.) So it’s actually possible that low back pain is the single most common reason that people seek help.
  28. I will cover this in more detail later, but the main citation for this is a 2017 paper by James Steele, PhD. He makes the case for this in detail and uses it as a premise for the hypothesis that back pain probably has something to do with a basic vulnerability in human biology, something we all share. See “An evolutionary hypothesis to explain the role of deconditioning in low back pain prevalence in humans”.
  29. Nachemson says, “Rarely are diagnoses scientifically valid … .” And Deyo: “There are wide variations in care, a fact that suggests there is professional uncertainty about the optimal approach.” Many other researchers have made this point, but Sarno (p. 111) states it most eloquently:

    There is probably no other medical condition which is treated in so many different ways and by such a variety of practitioners as back pain. Though the conclusion may be uncomfortable, the medical community must bear the responsibility for this, for it has been distressingly narrow in its approach to the problem. It has been trapped by a diagnostic bias of ancient vintage and, most uncharacteristically, has uncritically accepted an unproven concept, that structural abnormalities are the cause of back pain.”

  30. Sarno J. Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain. Trade paperback, red/blue cover ed. Berkley Books; 1999. p. 27.
  31. Moseley L. Teaching people about pain — why do we keep beating around the bush? Pain Management. 2012;2(1):2–3. PubMed 24654610 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54762 ❐
  32. Battié MC, Videman T, Kaprio J, et al. The Twin Spine Study: contributions to a changing view of disc degeneration. Spine J. 2009;9(1):47–59. PubMed 19111259 ❐ The once commonly held view that disc degeneration is primarily a result of aging and wear and tear from mechanical insults and injuries was not supported by this series of studies. Instead, disc degeneration appears to be determined in great part by genetic influences. Although environmental factors also play a role, it is not primarily through routine physical loading exposures (eg, heavy vs. light physical demands) as once suspected.”
  33. Dr. Deyo is quoted here by the New York Times in Back Pain Spending Surge Shows No Benefit regarding this paper published in Journal of the American Medical Association: “Expenditures and Health Status Among Adults With Back and Neck Problems”
  34. Chou R, Qaseem A, Snow V, et al. Diagnosis and Treatment of Low Back Pain: A Joint Clinical Practice Guideline from the American College of Physicians and the American Pain Society. Ann Intern Med. 2007 Oct 2;147(7):478–491. PubMed 17909209 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 56029 ❐The American College of Physicians and the American Pain Society “strongly” recommend that doctors avoid trying to diagnose back pain with MRI and X-rays unless symptoms persist and/or include severe neurological problems. For my report when the guidelines were announced, see MRI and X-Ray Often Worse than Useless for Back Pain
  35. Gina Kolata for the New York Times:

    The pitchers were not injured and had no pain. But the M.R.I.’s found abnormal shoulder cartilage in 90 percent of them and abnormal rotator cuff tendons in 87 percent. “If you want an excuse to operate on a pitcher’s throwing shoulder, just get an M.R.I.,” Dr. Andrews says.

    And …

    “It is very rare for an M.R.I. to come back with the words ‘normal study,’ “ said Dr. Christopher DiGiovanni, a professor of orthopedics and a sports medicine specialist at Brown University. “I can’t tell you the last time I’ve seen it.”

    No kidding. I’m not sure I’ve ever had a conversation with someone about their MRI that didn’t involve speculating about the significance of something the scan revealed.

  36. Pham HH, Landon BE, Reschovsky JD, Wu B, Schrag D. Rapidity and Modality of Imaging for Acute Low Back Pain in Elderly Patients. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169(10):972–981. PainSci Bibliography 55430 ❐

    “Well-established guidelines indicate that rapid or advanced imaging is not beneficial in the absence of specific complicating features,” and imaging is “rarely indicated, even for elderly patients.”

    In spite of that, however, this large study of 35,000 (!) patients with low back pain found that almost 30% were given a X-ray or MRI within 28 days. Black and older patients received less rapid imaging, which probably indicates that a lot of imaging is driven by a profit motive: that is, it’s sold to more affluent clients.

    “Patients may consider imaging reassuring, and those with higher socioeconomic status may be more successful in obtaining testing in this context. However, in contrast to generally underused services such as diabetic monitoring, more rapid or advanced imaging for low back pain may not benefit patients and may result in harm.”

  37. Carey TS, Garrett J, Project NCB. Patterns of ordering diagnostic tests for patients with acute low back pain. Ann Intern Med. 1996 Nov 15;125(10):807–14. PubMed 8928987 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 57065 ❐

    From the abstract: “Radiography is commonly used as a diagnostic test for patients with acute back pain.”

  38. Swedlow A, Johnson G, Smithline N, Milstein A. Increased costs and rates of use in the California workers’ compensation system as a result of self-referral by physicians. N Engl J Med. 1992;327:1502–6. PubMed 1406882 ❐

    From the abstract: “Of all the MRI scans requested by the self-referring physicians, 38 percent were found to be medically inappropriate … ”

    Self-referring physicians are physicians sending patients to pain clinics or imaging facilities that they own: that is, referring patients to themselves (“You need to buy more testing from me.”).

  39. Deyo RA, Weinstein DO. Low Back Pain. N Engl J Med. 2001 Feb;344(5):363–70. PubMed 11172169 ❐ “ … imaging is often unnecessary.”
  40. Chou R, Fu R, Carrino JA, Deyo RA. Imaging strategies for low-back pain: systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet. 2009 Feb;373(9662):463–72. PubMed 19200918 ❐

    “Some clinicians do lumbar imaging routinely or in the absence of historical or clinical features suggestive of serious low-back problems,” but this review of six studies of the subject clearly concludes that they really should not do that. It simply does no good, but it does waste resources and scare patients. As long as there are no signs of a serious underlying condition, “lumbar imaging for low back pain … does not improve clinical outcomes.”

  41. Chou R, Qaseem A, Owens DK, Shekelle P; for the Clinical Guidelines Committee of the American College of Physicians. Diagnostic Imaging for Low Back Pain: Advice for High-Value Health Care From the American College of Physicians. Ann Intern Med. 2011 Feb;154(3):181–189. PubMed 21282698 ❐

    From the abstract: “ … evidence indicates that routine imaging is not associated with clinically meaningful benefits but can lead to harms. … In this area, more testing does not equate to better care.” Emphasis emphatically mine.

  42. Brinjikji W, Luetmer PH, Comstock B, et al. Systematic Literature Review of Imaging Features of Spinal Degeneration in Asymptomatic Populations. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2015 Apr;36(4):811–6. PubMed 25430861 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53872 ❐ Signs of degeneration are present in high percentages of healthy people with no symptoms … even young people. For instance, asymptomatic disc degeneration is present in 68% of 40-year-olds. Painless disc bulges are present in 40% of 30-year-olds. The authors concluded: “Many imaging-based degenerative features are likely part of normal aging and unassociated with pain.”
  43. Deyo RA, Weinstein DO. Low Back Pain. N Engl J Med. 2001 Feb;344(5):363–70. PubMed 11172169 ❐ “ … disk and other abnormalities are common among asymptomatic adults.”
  44. Boden SD, Davis DO, Dina TS, Patronas NJ, Wiesel SW. Abnormal magnetic-resonance scans of the lumbar spine in asymptomatic subjects. A prospective investigation. J Bone Joint Surg Am. 1990;72(3):403–408.

    The authors found that 22% of pain-free adults under 60 had herniated discs. A whopping 93% of asymptomatic volunteers over 60 had signs of disk degeneration.

  45. Jensen MC, Brant-Zawadzki MN, Obuchowski N, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of the lumbar spine in people without back pain. N Engl J Med. 1994;331(2):69–73. PubMed 8208267 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 56640 ❐

    The authors found 28% of healthy adults with no low back pain had a herniated disc, and 52% had a disk bulge! They conclude: “On MRI examination of the lumbar spine, many people without back pain have disk bulges or protrusions but not extrusions. Given the high prevalence of these findings and of back pain, the discovery by MRI of bulges or protrusions in people with low back pain may frequently be coincidental.”

  46. Weishaupt D, Zanetti M, Hodler J, Boos N. MR imaging of the lumbar spine: prevalence of intervertebral disk extrusion and sequestration, nerve root compression, end plate abnormalities, and osteoarthritis of the facet joints in asymptomatic volunteers. Radiology. 1998;209(3):661–666.

    This research found that an incredible 40% of asymptomatic adults had herniated discs, and 72% had signs of degeneration. The mean age of the sample of 60 adults was 35.

  47. Stadnik TW, Lee RR, Coen HL, et al. Annular tears and disk herniation: prevalence and contrast enhancement on MR images in the absence of low back pain or sciatica. Radiology. 1998 Jan;206(1):49–55. PubMed 9423651 ❐

    29 of 36 asymptomatic people nevertheless had “bulging disk and focal disk protrusion.” From the abstract: “Annular tears and focal disk protrusions on MR images ... are frequently found in an asymptomatic population.” Indeed, these researchers found that a whopping 81% of pain-free adults had bulging disks, and 56% had annular tears (ripping of connective tissue near the disk).

    While that is interesting, it is also worth noting that these signs are at the milder end of the spectrum of pathological possibilities for discs, and therefore it is less surprising that they would be asymptomatic.

  48. Borenstein DG, JW O’Mara SDB, et al. The value of magnetic resonance imaging of the lumbar spine to predict low-back pain in asymptomatic subjects. Journal of Bone & Joint Surgery (American). 2001;83:1306–1311. PubMed 11568190 ❐

    In 1989, 21 of 67 perfectly healthy people, with no history of back pain, were scanned with MRI, and found to have “an identifiable abnormality of a disc or of the spinal canal.” Such findings are surprising, but perhaps the effect of such abnormalities is delayed: how were they doing a decade later? The research team followed up with 50 of the original subjects.

    Their backs looked somewhat worse — more of the same — but only 21 of them had developed back pain, and not the same 21 that had abnormalities in ‘89. Only 12 of them had any findings on their original scans (and some of those were trivial).

    The authors therefore concluded that “the findings on magnetic resonance scans were not predictive of the development or duration of low-back pain” and that “clinical correlation is essential to determine the importance of abnormalities on magnetic resonance images.” MRI findings — and the structural abnormalities that they reveal — are essentially meaningless on their own.

  49. Ong A, Anderson J, Roche J. A pilot study of the prevalence of lumbar disc degeneration in elite athletes with lower back pain at the Sydney 2000 Olympic Games. Br J Sports Med. 2003 Jun;37(3):263–6. PubMed 12782554 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 56840 ❐

    The primary result of this small study of the spines of elite athletes with back pain was that they have “significantly greater” signs of spinal degeneration for their age than ordinary folks. However, it also showed that a substantial percentage of them had back pain despite having no visible signs of degeneration, or only quite minor ones.

    My point here is that half of them did not have structural problems, in spite of their symptoms. These are elite athletes, not malingerers! If they say they hurt, they hurt. Yet MRI failed to identify a problem in half of them.
  50. Deyo RA, Weinstein DO. Low Back Pain. N Engl J Med. 2001 Feb;344(5):363–70. PubMed 11172169 ❐ “The association between symptoms and imaging results is weak.”
  51. Beattie PF, Meyers SP. Magnetic resonance imaging in low back pain: general principles and clinical issues. Phys Ther. 1998 Jul;78(7):738–53. PubMed 9672546 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 56987 ❐
  52. For several years now, it’s been progressive — maybe almost fashionable — for back pain educators to trash talk imaging the way I am here. But just because we can’t seem to reliably find smoking gun causes of back pain with modern imaging technology does not mean it isn’t there. Hancock et al argue that “diagnostic studies should include and investigate pathological sources of low back pain. … A better understanding of the biological component of low back pain in relation, and in addition, to psychosocial factors is important for a more rational approach to management of low back pain.”
  53. Deyo et al report that disc herniations account for about 4% of all back pain, and Sarno argues at length that nearly all disc herniations are incidental to the clinical problem, a sideshow that doesn’t do much except make people anxious. Yet approximately three quarters of the back pain patients that I see have either been “diagnosed” with a disc herniation, or they are concerned about the possibility!
  54. Zhong M, Liu JT, Jiang H, et al. Incidence of Spontaneous Resorption of Lumbar Disc Herniation: A Meta-Analysis. Pain Physician. 2017;20(1):E45–E52. PubMed 28072796 ❐
  55. Chiu CC, Chuang TY, Chang KH, et al. The probability of spontaneous regression of lumbar herniated disc: a systematic review. Clin Rehabil. 2015 Feb;29(2):184–95. PubMed 25009200 ❐

    Not only do many lumbar disc herniations (and extrusions and so on) resolve on their own, or with just a little help from conservative therapy, but the worse the herniation the more likely it is to spontaneously regress! This is exactly the opposite of what common sense predicts. This systematic review of thirty studies reveals a strong pattern of better regression from the worst cases:

    “Patients with disc extrusion and sequestration had a significantly higher possibility of having spontaneous regression than did those with bulging or protruding discs.”

  56. Kjaer P, Tunset A, Boyle E, Jensen TS. Progression of lumbar disc herniations over an eight-year period in a group of adult Danes from the general population: a longitudinal MRI study using quantitative measures. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2016 Jan 15;17(1):26. PubMed 26767364 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53406 ❐

    Impressively, this is “the first study to investigate changes in the size of lumbar disc herniations” over a long period, using reliable objective measurements taken with MRI. They took three scans of 140 herniated intervertebral discs in 106 people at ages 41, 45, and 49. Subjects were excluded if they didn’t have herniations at both the first and second scans. The researchers looked at changes and correlations between protrusion size, disc height, and cross-sectional area.

    The results showed that disc herniations are mostly quite stable: about 65% of them got no worse or better. However, 17.5% of intervertebral disc herniations do spontaneously resolve on their own, and only 12.5% got worse. The small remainder of 5% fluctuated. Larger herniations were predictive of reductions in area and height. Five scans would have been a nice improvement to the design, but even from three we can see that, clearly, “lumbar disk herniation has an uncertain natural history” (Hong). Indeed.

    Those numbers are not awesome numbers — clearly herniations do not all just magically go away — but I do think they are different and much less discouraging numbers than most people have in their heads.

    (No one knows how this works, by the way. Why would a disc herniation spontaneously resolve? Or pop in and out of its proper place like a gopher? It’s a mystery.)

  57. NEJM.org [Internet]. Hong J, Ball PA. Resolution of Lumbar Disk Herniation without Surgery; 2016 Apr 21 [cited 23 May 2]. PainSci Bibliography 53401 ❐

    “Lumbar disk herniation has an uncertain natural history.” UNDERSTATEMENT. Check out these before/after pictures of a disk herniation that solved itself. Look closely where the arrow is pointing. Read the single paragraph description of the case. No scalpels were involved in this recovery. Now, did your concern level about disc herniations just drop 3 notches? Good, mission accomplished. File under “back pain rarely as bad as it feels.”

  58. el Barzouhi A, Vleggeert-Lankamp CLA, Lycklama à Nijeholt GJ, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging in follow-up assessment of sciatica. N Engl J Med. 2013 Mar;368(11):999–1007. PubMed 23484826 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54565 ❐

    Disc, schmisc: patients with back pain and sciatica recovered about equally well with or without disc herniations visible on MRI. Most (84%) recovered well within a year … and there were actually 2% more good outcomes in the patients with disc herniations! Or as physical therapist and sciatica expert Tom Jesson put it, “Of all the people who still had a disc herniation a year after treatment, 85% felt better.”

    This seems like a surprising result, but it’s what the research has been pointing to for years. The nearly identical stats could be a fluke, of course, but they’d have to be off by a lot to change the reassuring take-home message. Even a 20-point difference, ten times larger than this, would still show that a “slipped disc” confirmed by MRI isn’t nearly as worrisome as most people assume.

    Two other notable findings, further emphasizing how uninformative MRI can be: “distinguishing between protrusions and extrusions did not have diagnostic value” and “we did not find a positive correlation between the presence of scar tissue and symptoms.”

  59. Herzog R, Elgort DR, Flanders AE, Moley PJ. Variability in diagnostic error rates of 10 MRI centers performing lumbar spine MRI examinations on the same patient within a 3-week period. Spine J. 2016 Nov. PubMed 27867079 ❐
  60. Peul WC, van den Hout WB, Brand R, et al. Prolonged conservative care versus early surgery in patients with sciatica caused by lumbar disc herniation: two year results of a randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2008 Jun;336(7657):1355–8. PubMed 18502911 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53367 ❐ The authors of this paper compared conservative therapy to surgery for sciatica and concluded that, “Neither treatment is clearly preferable.” They argued, “It might therefore be time to shift from the current situation of physicians’ recommendations about the need for surgery (often based on their personal preferences) to patients deciding, with the help of their physician, which treatment strategy is best for them.”
  61. Deyo RA, Weinstein DO. Low Back Pain. N Engl J Med. 2001 Feb;344(5):363–70. PubMed 11172169 ❐ Deyo’s paper is a tutorial for physicians. He comments in the first paragraph that “ … there is evidence of excessive imaging and surgery for low back pain in the United States, and many experts believe that the problem has been ‘overmedicalized.’” This opinion is supported with references to Carey, Swedlow, and Waddell, among others. North American physicians are looking for mechanical problems, and they are trying to solve them with surgery.
  62. There are many ways to demonstrate this. The next few footnotes cover several different angles. For instance, a 2008 study in Journal of the American Medical Association that showed that “spine-related expenditures have increased substantially from 1997 to 2005, without evidence of corresponding improvement in self-assessed health status” (see Martin). In other words, a lot of expensive medical care is not helping. This interesting paper was summarized well by Tara Parker-Pope in the New York Times.
  63. Not all surgery is ineffective (see Alvarez, for instance, who concludes about a common spinal surgery, decompressive laminectomy, “It is fairly clear, however, that in most patients with clear radiographic and clinical evidence of stenosis, decompressive surgery provides significant relief.”) However, there are conspicuous problems with spinal surgery, chiefly that we must question the rationale of any surgery that is done to correct any of the spinal structural problems that often do not cause symptoms. Until we understand why as many as 40% of adults may be walking around with painless herniated discs (Weishaupt), it seems unwise to assume that a herniated disc is necessarily the cause of any patient’s low back pain — or to operate based on that assumption!
  64. Gibson JN, Grant IC, Waddell G. The Cochrane review of surgery for lumbar disc prolapse and degenerative lumbar spondylosis. Spine. 1999 Sep 1;24(17):1820–32. PubMed 10488513 ❐ From the abstract: “There is no scientific evidence on the effectiveness of any form of surgical decompression or fusion for degenerative lumbar spondylosis compared with natural history, placebo, or conservative management.”
  65. Deyo et al, op. cit. “There is no evidence from clinical trials or cohort studies that surgery is effective for patients who have low back pain unless they have sciatica, pseudoclaudication, or spondylolisthesis.” Yet there are, of course, numerous surgical procedures intended to correct other kinds of structural problems presumed to be the cause of back pain.
  66. Deyo et al, op. cit. With regards to treating stenosis with surgery (usually regarded as an effective procedure, again see Alvarez), Deyo writes, “Even with successful surgery, symptoms often recur after several years. At four years of postoperative follow-up, about 30 percent of patients have severe pain and about 10 percent have undergone re-operation.” This is supported with references to Atlas as well as another of his own papers, [Deyo 1993].
  67. The placebo problem is simple: Some (or perhaps even many) successful spinal surgeries are successful not because they’ve corrected a mechanical problem, but because the patient is (extremely) impressed by the seriousness of the procedure and feels unusually optimistic. There will be much more information about this important issue later in the tutorial: see The back surgery placebo problem.
  68. Structurally, the spine has at least one distinct advantage over most other musculoskeletal structures in the body: it has a modular design. That is, it is composed of multiple standardized units. Primary spinal functions can be carried out by any significant subset of those units. Unlike your knees, for instance. If you seriously damage a knee joint, you lose all knee function. If you seriously damage a spinal joint, several other spinal joints can produce almost identical spinal function (see the next note). Also, although the spine is complex, evidence abounds that it is actually quite sturdy. Spinal joint dislocations and vertebral fractures are rare, occurring much less often than they do in other parts of the body. Such injuries require enormous physical trauma, like what happens in car accidents — and even then the majority of injuries affect the cervical spine. And no wonder: the spine in general — and the lumbar spine in particular — is an amazing structural compromise between flexibility and stability. Lumbar joints move just enough to give the whole lumbar spine considerable elastic qualities, yet they are entwined so thoroughly that no single lumbar joint moves much at all. And these well-balanced features are strongly consistent with what you’d expect from a structure that has evolved to protect the most crucial of all anatomy, the spinal cord. Lumbar spinal joints are not invulnerable, but they are certainly sturdier than people tend to think they are!
  69. Niemeyer T, Bovingloh AS, Halm H, Liljenqvist U. Results after anterior-posterior lumbar spinal fusion: 2-5 years follow-up. Int Orthop. 2004 Oct;28(5):298–302. PubMed 15480660 ❐

    Although this research concludes that anterior-posterior lumbar spinal fusion is an effective treatment for chronic low back pain, in my opinion the methodology here is poor — essentially just a bunch of self-serving anecdotes — and the results are unconvincing.

    My own interpretation is quite different: the surgical procedure may have been irrelevant to the observed improvement in symptoms, which could well have occurred without the surgery. However, the results do demonstrate another important point: that the spine is not fragile and it is possible for people who’ve had chronic low back pain to experience an improvement in symptoms in spite of spinal fusion.

  70. This is an interesting one requiring a bit more explanation than just a citation. Its relevance to back pain may not be immediately obvious to the untrained eye. The evidence comes from a 2010 paper in British Journal of Sports Medicine (Hides et al), and appears to be about Australian Rules Football players and hip muscles. But hips are highly relevant to the low back, particularly these ones — the psoas major muscles are two of the four big columns of muscle that surround and brace the lumbar spine. The research also looked at the quadratus lumborum muscle, which is much more straightforwardly a back muscle.

    Researchers used MRI to measure the size of these muscles in 54 AFL players — very serious athletes, these guys — and found that “asymmetry of the psoas and the quadratus lumborum muscles exists in elite players.” That is, they aren’t “balanced.”

    Such asymmetries are widely believed by therapists to be clinically significant. Manual therapists, if they suspected such a distinct asymmetry in muscle mass — and they often do — would enthusiastically and almost unanimously embrace this significant lack of “balance” as a major risk factor for injuries in the region, and certainly as a likely suspect in your back pain. Therefore, they would also almost certainly recommend (expensive, time-consuming) therapy based on this idea: stretching and manipulation for the “over”-developed side, strengthening for the other side, and so on. I have seen patients spend literally thousands of dollars and many months of regular therapy and workouts trying to “solve” muscle imbalances.

    However, the researchers also found that “asymmetry in muscle size was not related to number of injuries.” Not. Related. The most imbalanced players were no more likely to have a problem than the most balanced players. End of story. Just another great example of exaggerating the importance of how crooked you are.

  71. Maurer M, Soder RB, Baldisserotto M. Spine abnormalities depicted by magnetic resonance imaging in adolescent rowers. Am J Sports Med. 2011 Feb;39(2):392–7. PubMed 20889986 ❐

    Rowers often experience abnormalities of the lumbar spine as a result of their sport, but those abnormalities are not strongly associated with symptoms.

  72. Fox AJ, Lin JP,, Pinto RS, Kricheff II. Myelographic cervical nerve root deformities. Radiology. 1975 Aug;116(02):355–61. PubMed 168615 ❐

    Although this was a study of the cervical spine, its findings are certainly relevant to the back as well. Paraphrasing, Sarno (p21) writes: “ … even large growths in the neck, such as benign tumours, produce no pain.” From the abstract: “Good correlation between specific features of root deformities and clinical significance could not be demonstrated.” In other words, nerve root deformity itself does not constitute a nerve root problem. If a deformed nerve root works and doesn’t hurt, it’s not a problem!

  73. Luoma K, Vehmas T, Raininko R, et al. Lumbosacral transitional vertebra: relation to disc degeneration and low back pain. Spine. 2004 Jan 15;29(2):200–5. PubMed 1472241 ❐

    From the abstract, “Lumbosacral transitional vertebra increases the risk of early degeneration in the upper disc,” yet “transitional vertebra is not associated with any type of LBP.”

  74. Magora A, Schwartz A. Relation between low back pain and X-ray changes 4: Lysis and olisthesis. Scand J Rehabil Med. 1980;12(2):47–52. PubMed 6451925 ❐

    From the abstract, “No direct relation between sacralization, partial or complete, and LBP was found.”

  75. Magora A, Schwartz A. Relation between the low back pain syndrome and x-ray findings 2: Transitional vertebra (mainly sacralization). Scand J Rehabil Med. 1978;10(3):135–45. PubMed 151915 ❐

    From the abstract, “No relation between prelysis and lysis, and low back pain was found. Lysis seemed to be associated with a higher severity of low back pain. All the subjects with olisthesis suffered from low back pain.”

  76. Magora A, Schwartz A. Relation between the low back pain syndrome and x-ray findings 3: Spina bifida occulta. Scand J Rehabil Med. 1980;12(1):9–15. PubMed 644614 ❐

    From the abstract, “It is concluded that spina bifida occulta does not play a causative role, does not cause a proneness to LBP and does not influence the chronicity of LBP.”

  77. Splithoff AJ. Lumbosacral junction; roentgenographic comparison of patients with and without backaches. JAMA. 1953 Aug 22;152(17):1610–3. PubMed 13069226 ❐

    This is one of the earliest examples of research showing poor correlation between structural problems and back pain. Sarno summarizes in Mind Over Back Pain (p23): “Splithoff compared the occurrence of nine different abnormalities of the lower end of the spine in patients with and without back pain. He concluded that patients without backache demonstrated structural aberrations just as frequently as patients with back pain.”

  78. Buchbinder R, Osborne RH, Ebeling PR, et al. A Randomized Trial of Vertebroplasty for Painful Osteoporotic Vertebral Fractures. N Engl J Med. 2009 Aug 6;361(6):557–568. PainSci Bibliography 55333 ❐

    This is one of a pair of 2009 papers (see also Kallmes et al) presenting strong evidence that there is “no beneficial effect” to stabilizing fractured spines with injections of bone cement (vertebroplasty), a common and yet apparently dubious procedure. That evidence is backed up by major reviews published since (Buchbinder 2015, Stevenson 2014), but some contrary evidence has also been published (Shi 2012, Klazen), and it is possible vertebroplasty works better for some kinds of patients.

    Nevertheless, it’s a major comeuppance for a seemingly straightforward surgical fix that should have been tested more carefully long ago. And if stabilizing the spine with cement doesn’t resolve symptoms, it strongly suggests that instability wasn’t the problem to begin with. The rationale for vertebroplasty has always been cave-man simple: Ooog. Verteba [sic] busted. Hurt. Thag make bone stronger. Inject glue. Ugh. Supposedly these fractures are painful because the spine is unstable — hardly an unreasonable assumption — and therefore stabilizing them will help. Except it didn’t!

    (See more detailed commentary on this paper.)

  79. Andrade NS, Ashton CM, Wray NP, Brown C, Bartanusz V. Systematic review of observational studies reveals no association between low back pain and lumbar spondylolysis with or without isthmic spondylolisthesis. Eur Spine J. 2015 Jun;24(6):1289–95. PubMed 25833204 ❐

    It is widely believed that spondylolysis (SL) and/or isthmic spondylolisthesis (IS) cause low back pain. If so, individuals with these conditions should be more prone to back pain (duh). This paper reviewed other studies looking for that association. They found 15 adequate studies. None of them detected an association between SL/IS and LBP. The authors speculate that the two apparent benefits of treatments may just be “due to benign natural history and nonspecific treatment effects.” They suggest that “traditional surgical practice … should be reconsidered.”

  80. Sarno J. Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain. Trade paperback, red/blue cover ed. Berkley Books; 1999. Sarno is the only expert I know of who argues that spondylolisthesis and stenosis do not cause low back pain even when they are obviously present: “I have had a number of patients with this abnormality and have found in each case that the patient also had TMS . In every case the pain was eliminated by proper treatment for TMS, suggesting that spondylolisthesis was not the cause” (p25). This is followed by a “dramatic case history.” With regards to stenosis, “ … it has been my experience this ‘abnormality’ is rarely responsible for leg or back pain” (p27). For my part, for now I accept that these two conditions may cause pain, but not inevitably. New evidence supports this opinion — see the next note!
  81. Haig AJ, Tong HC, Yamakawa KS, et al. Spinal stenosis, back pain, or no symptoms at all? A masked study comparing radiologic and electrodiagnostic diagnoses to the clinical impression. Archives of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation. 2006 Jul;87(7):897–903. PubMed 16813774 ❐

    In this study, about 150 people were assessed for back pain in different ways, including MRI, but “radiologic and clinical impression had no relation.” In other words, there was no useful similarity between evaluation of the patient with MRI, and evaluation by examination and taking a history. “The impression obtained from an MRI scan does not determine whether lumbar stenosis is a cause of pain.” Since MRI does in fact identify narrowing of the spinal canal, and this is the whole basis of diagnosing spinal stenosis with MRI, these results also strongly imply that a narrowed spinal canal does not (alone) cause back pain.

  82. Akhaddar A, Boucetta M. Dislocation of the Cervical Spine. N Engl J Med. 2010 May 20;362(1920). PainSci Bibliography 55145 ❐

    A case report (and disturbing X-ray) of a traumatic cervical spine dislocation, notable for being mostly asymptomatic: just torticollis and limited motion, but no pain, weakness or altered sensation. That such a serious injury can have so little impact on a person is quite interesting!

  83. Moon SJ, Lee JK, Seo BR, Kim SH. Traumatic subluxation associated with absent cervical pedicle: case report and review of the literature. Spine. 2008 Aug;33(18):E663–6. PubMed 18708921 ❐

    This paper describes the case of a man who developed severe neck and shoulder pain after a fall. It turned out that a part of one of his neck vertebrae was entirely missing, since birth (“a relatively uncommon developmental anomaly”). The fall subluxed the joint substantially. Although painful, the lack of neurological symptoms is surprising — and more evidence that nerve roots are not easily pinched.

  84. College of Physicians and Surgeons of the Province of Quebec. The scientific brief against chiropractic. The New Physician, Sep 1966.
  85. Deyo., op. cit. “Low back pain affects men and women equally, with onset most often between 30 and 50 years.”
  86. Robinson GE. A combined approach to a medical problem: The Canadian Back Education Unit. Can J Psychiatry. 1980 Mar;25(2):138–42. PubMed 6447538 ❐

    This Canadian study of 2200 people with back pain showed a surprising distribution of ages: back pain after age fifty is much less common than it is between thirty and fifty. Back pain in the teens and twenties is rarer still.

  87. Could it be that young people get more back pain because they are more active and more likely to injure themselves? Perhaps, but there are some problems with this theory. Mainly, few back injuries occur during common athletic activity — I can’t recall a single case of back pain in my office that started with a clearly identifiable trauma during running, swimming, cycling, or skiing. It’s usually something more like “I was lifting my baby daughter,” which, as far as I know, is a problem for both parents and grandparents.
  88. Hadjipavlou AG, Tzermiadianos MN, Bogduk N, Zindrick MR. The pathophysiology of disc degeneration: a critical review. Journal of Bone & Joint Surgery (British Volume). 2008 Oct;90(10):1261–70. PubMed 18827232 ❐ From the abstract: “There is no clear evidence indicating whether ageing in the presence of repetitive injury or repetitive injury in the absence of ageing plays a greater role in the degenerative process.”
  89. Here’s the raw data from Harkness. The data in the graph is averaged and smoothed.

    age muscle_pain women
    18–24 0.0% 5.6%
    25–34 1.7% 5.7%
    35–44 8.6% 9.8%
    45–54 15.2% 13.2%
    55–64 7.1% 7.5%
  90. Battié 2009, op. cit.
  91. Deyo., op. cit. “Perhaps 85% of patients with isolated low back pain cannot be given a precise pathoanatomical diagnosis … ” Yet we also know that about 70% of back pain diagnoses made by physicians are “strain” or “sprain”! Deyo goes on to say that “Strain and sprain have never been anatomically or histologically characterized, and patients given these diagnoses might accurately be said to have idiopathic low back pain.” In other words, between 70 and 85% of low back pain is unexplained — yet is routinely attributed to familiar-sounding injuries without any evidence that this is the case. Doubleplusungood!
  92. Baron R, Binder A. How neuropathic is sciatica? The mixed pain concept. Orthopade. 2004 May;33(5):568–75. PubMed 15067505 ❐

    Sciatica is poorly understood. From the abstract: “The incidence of each pain component in chronic sciatica as well as validated diagnostic tools to identify them remain unknown.”

  93. Vlaeyen JWS, Maher CG, Wiech K, et al. Low back pain. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2018 Dec;4(1):52. PubMed 30546064 ❐

    This is a thorough chronic low back pain treatment primer for clinicians. The writing is dry but clear, and although a lot of expert opinion is interjected, there’s also an obvious effort to be rigorously evidence-based. There were very few red flags for me as a skeptic.

    The paper focusses on “the development of new diagnostic procedures, evidence-based screening methods and more targeted interventions,” and broadly conclude that there’s a need for “a multidisciplinary approach to the management of low back pain that integrates biological, psychological and social aspects.” Too broadly, I think: the paper doesn’t really deliver much in the way of better diagnosis or more “targeted” treatment, and in fact I found the treatment content to be the most anemic part of the paper.

    The authors certainly do a good job of explaining why the “injury model” isn’t adequate anymore, but there’s nothing new about that (back pain experts have been debunking the injury model since the 90s). And their review of mechanisms of pain is quite good.

  94. Dionne CE, Dunn KM, Croft PR. Does back pain prevalence really decrease with increasing age? A systematic review. Age Ageing. 2006 May;35(3):229–34. PubMed 16547119 ❐
  95. Bogduk N. What’s in a name? The labelling of back pain. Medical Journal of Australia. 2000;173(8):400–401. PainSci Bibliography 56046 ❐
  96. Hancock MJ, Maher CG, Latimer J, et al. Systematic review of tests to identify the disc, SIJ or facet joint as the source of low back pain. Eur Spine J. 2007 Oct;16(10):1539–50. PubMed 17566796 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55023 ❐
  97. In other words, if you have a very troubled disc that is the legit source of pain, an MRI can come close to confirming that by showing the disc. Even that’s not a slam dunk, but it is compelling combo to have both “symptoms of a disc herniation” and “hey, look, there’s also a disc herniation on the MRI!” But an MRI cannot show that there isn’t a disc problem — MRI is too fallible, and disc herniations can be surprisingly symptomatic even when minor.
  98. The tests were primarily “Revel’s criteria:” 5 or more of 7 clinical characteristics defined by Dr. Revel. I’d list them, but … they don’t diagnose anything!
  99. The kinds of tests that we’re talking about here are mostly what we call “special orthopedic tests,” most of which are “provocation tests” — basically, trying to make the body hurt by moving and poking the body in creative ways to determine what hurts and why. It’s an art as much as it’s a science, and many such tests have proven to be unreliable due to a wide range of flaws — most conspicuously the distressingly variable skills and wisdom with which they are applied and interpreted. Just one example: Cattrysse showed that two of three popular tests for a serious condition of instability in the upper cervical spine showed “no tendency” to get the same results when performed by different clinicians (while the other test was only “somewhat” reliable). And such tests done in “real life” will rarely be done as carefully or formally as they are done in a controlled scientific study, and if the tests can’t be shown to be reliable even there … well, don’t expect them to be all that informative on the front lines of patient care! For this reason, I dramatically scaled back my use of orthopedic tests in the later years of my massage career, taking even the most obvious findings from the most apparently simple tests with a grain of salt.
  100. Bogduk N. Management of chronic low back pain. Med J Aust. 2004;180(2):79–83.
  101. Chou R, Loeser JD, Owens DK, et al. Interventional therapies, surgery, and interdisciplinary rehabilitation for low back pain: an evidence-based clinical practice guideline from the American Pain Society. Spine. 2009 May;34(10):1066–1077. PubMed 19363457 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55429 ❐
  102. King W, Lau P, Lees R, Bogduk N. The validity of manual examination in assessing patients with neck pain. Spine Journal. 2007;7(1):22–26.
  103. Maigne JY, Cornelis P, Chatellier G. Lower back pain and neck pain: is it possible to identify the painful side by palpation only? Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2012 Mar;55(2):103–11. PubMed 22341057 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54321 ❐

    The results are obviously underwhelming. Although they did a little better than just guessing, the results suggest that it’s difficult even for expert examiners to detect the location of neck and back pain by feel. As well, they were only attempting to detect the side of pain — kind of the palpation equivalent of hitting the broad side of a barn in target practice. Imagine how much worse their performance would have been if they had to identify the location more precisely, or if the pain could have been anywhere or nowhere. So they barely passed the easiest possible test, and probably would have failed a harder one and done no better than guessing.

    An obvious weakness of the study is that only two examiners were tested. More and possibly more experienced examiners might have yielded different results. But one would still hope for better than this from anyone with any training and experience at all.

  104. Brinjikji W, Diehn FE, Jarvik JG, et al. MRI Findings of Disc Degeneration are More Prevalent in Adults with Low Back Pain than in Asymptomatic Controls: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2015 Dec;36(12):2394–9. PubMed 26359154 ❐
  105. Well, not entirely. I never chalked it up to “just” inexplicable back pain. I was well aware that there probably was something specific — I just didn’t know what it was. I’m pleased to say that my “instincts” were excellent with this case (AKA “a lot of experience and knowledge”). While I’m sure I have screwed up and missed warning signs with other patients, I did well here: even though there were no obvious signs, everything about the case just screamed “be careful, this is not one of the usual suspects.” In particular, I’m proud of myself for resisting the temptation to “just try rubbing aching muscles and see what happens.” I’ve done that with so many patients that it’s entirely possible it might have become a reflex, a lazy habit. But no, fortunately I knew better — I was paying attention to the right clues, and avoided making Alex worse and giving him yet another reason to be frustrated with healthcare professionals.
  106. Aota Y. Entrapment of middle cluneal nerves as an unknown cause of low back pain. World J Orthop. 2016 Mar;7(3):167–70. PubMed 27004164 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53097 ❐
  107. For example, maybe the real problem was not that she had an entrapped nerve, but that she had a modest vitamin D deficiency that made her progressively more vulnerable to the cluneal nerve entrapment that would have otherwise remained asymptomatic. It might have gone completely unnoticed, forever, without that subtle biological X factor. It wouldn’t be shocking if something like this was actually going on. And it’s important to consider these possibilities!

  108. BackSitPro.com [Internet]. McGill S. There is no such thing as “non-specific back pain”; [unknown] [cited 17 Apr 23]. PainSci Bibliography 53611 ❐
  109. Seriously! Thanks to the bonanza of COVID research, we now have a better understanding of what was always clear: viral infections have a significant aftermath of elevated inflammation affect nerves, muscles, and joints, causing significantly elevated pain for weeks, months, or perhaps even years. See Cui, Aschman, and Suh. The existence of this phenomenon is hardly proof in itself that some case of back pain are attributable to elevated systemic inflammation, but it is a good (and kinda freaky) example of how plausible it is that the real culprit might be messy background biology causing significantly greater vulnerability for periods of our lives.
  110. In medical writing, it’s super important to be humble, and to remind readers of uncertainties — and they are thickest in this part of the book. The clinical importance of muscle pain is unproven. Although several experts have been suggesting it for a long time, we’re a long way from having a bunch of scientific papers to back up the claim. Right now it’s more like circumstantial evidence in a court room — emotionally compelling, but not rigorous. I believe that muscle dysfunction explains a lot more of low back pain than is usually suspected, but certainly not all of it. And just because structural problems are exaggerated doesn’t mean they don’t exist at all.
  111. Quintner JL, Bove GM, Cohen ML. A critical evaluation of the trigger point phenomenon. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2015 Mar;54(3):392–9. PubMed 25477053 ❐

    Quintner, Cohen, and Bove argue that the common picture of trigger points as lesions in muscle and soft tissue, spelled out most formally in Gerwin 2004, is “flawed both in reasoning and in science,” and that treatments (e.g. massage, needling) based on that idea produces results “indistinguishable from the placebo effect.” They believe that all of the biological evidence put forward over the years (like Shah 2008) is critically flawed in one way or another, while other evidence leads elsewhere, and so the old picture of trigger points “remains conjecture in the face of conflicting data.” They also point out that the theory is inappropriately treated like an established fact by a great many people.

    Not even these fierce critics of trigger points deny that people have pain that seems to come from their muscles. But if it’s not coming from the muscle, where is it coming from? They briefly discuss two other ideas of their own: inflamed nerve fibres, and referred pain and tenderness from deeper structures. They do not thoroughly explore or defend either idea.

    Neuritis is undoubtedly worth investigating, but it requires us to believe that nerve axons are routinely inflamed for no apparent reason, which doesn’t seem much different than the theory it is supposed to replace. The evidence cited to support it is just as limited as the evidence for trigger points, if not more so (just a few papers, all from the authors themselves, or their research colleagues).

    The proposal of “referred pain and tenderness” from deeper tissues with unspecified troubles and/or “altered central nociceptive mechanisms” is imprecise. We know that these mechanisms probably exist, but there is not a jot of evidence that they have anything to do with the subjective experience of “muscle pain.” This is just a proposal to look somewhere other than the integrated hypothesis.

    This paper is an abridged version of a much more detailed argument laid out in a chapter of an extremely expensive textbook: see Quintner.

  112. Quintner et al concede that people experience pain and sensitivity that seems to be in muscle tissue. The pain is real, and the need for an explanation and effective treatment is real:

    This is not to deny the existence of the clinical phenomena themselves, for which scientifically sound and logically plausible explanations based on known neurophysiological phenomena can be advanced.

  113. This point is based entirely on my own clinical observations as a massage therapist. Although I’m not aware of any hard data that can support it, it doesn’t seem any more necessary than it does to argue that cats like sunny windowsills. It’s obvious to any massage therapist: by far the most common client complaint is aching in the low back, and they practically all have clearly defined (“just a little higher!”) sensitive spots in their lumbar muscles. I’ll speculate a bit about why this might be below.
  114. I’m often asked what I think of a treatment that supposedly has a “100% success rate.” Or any percentage over 90, really. It’s the same answer, every time: hyperbolic treatment claims in health care are bollocks (and are almost always part of a sales pitch). Most painful problems are not really one problem. Perfect treatment results are nonsense due to co-morbidities & multiple overlapping etiologies alone. Nothing can treat everything. In fact, nothing can treat anything reliably.
  115. Evans RW. Book review of Muscle Pain: Understanding Its Nature, Diagnosis, and Treatment. N Engl J Med. 2001;344(13):1026–1027. PainSci Bibliography 57200 ❐

    This is a good review of an important text, Muscle Pain: Understanding its nature, diagnosis and treatment, that every physical therapist should read. I appreciate the review for its acknowledgement that, “Low back pain is of myofascial origin [in many cases].” The book defends the same idea at length. That doesn’t mean it’s right, of course, but the argument is substantive and serious.

  116. The MS hug is a collection of nasty symptoms caused by spasms in the intercostal muscles, often experienced long before diagnosis. Although the feeling of a tight band around the chest is the classic symptom, many patients also just experience widespread pain in the chest wall.
  117. “Presumably,” he says! That is an interesting assumption to examine, actually. I can imagine a variety of reasons why the sensitivity of muscle tissue might fluctuate over time. For instance, it could be a defensive system, where the sensitivity of the skin and muscle over the site of an injury is jacked up just to discourage you from pressing on it. Sounds good. But I don’t think it’s very likely, for three reasons. First, pressure on what seems like sore muscle is routinely extremely pleasant and satisfying, not alarming. Second, this kind of sensitivity routinely, usually occurs in the absence of any plausible underlying injury to protect, and this is particularly obvious in areas other than the back — and, conversely, it does not occur with many clear physical traumas. Third, this kind of sensitivity clearly seems to wax and wane in quite different rhythms than injury, coming and going in a much more mercurial fashion, or never going, or actually increasing as an injury wanes.

    So, yes, I can imagine a variety of reasons why muscle tissue might be sensitive … but I think “unhealthy in some way” is by far the most plausible. Just don’t ask me in what way…

  118. Graven-Nielsen T, Mense S, Arendt-Nielsen L. Painful and non-painful pressure sensations from human skeletal muscle. Experimental Brain Research. 2004 Dec;159(3):273–83. PubMed 15480607 ❐
  119. Many organs and tissues in the body are miraculously “busy” and complex, but muscle is characterized by particularly extreme and highly energetic performance, routinely functioning at the limits of what biology will allow, to the point where it’s often injured by the sheer intensity of its metabolic activity. See A Deep Dive into Delayed-Onset Muscle Soreness. All physiology is wondrous at the scale of organic chemistry, but the proteins that power muscle contraction are among the most dramatic examples of molecular “machines” in nature: see Micro Muscles and the Dance of the Sarcomeres: A mental picture of muscle knot physiology helps to explain four familiar features of muscle pain.
  120. Sarno J. Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain. Trade paperback, red/blue cover ed. Berkley Books; 1999. p. 75.
  121. Travell J, Simons D, Simons L. Myofascial Pain and Dysfunction: The Trigger Point Manual. 2nd ed. Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 1999.
  122. Shah JP, Danoff JV, Desai MJ, et al. Biochemicals associated with pain and inflammation are elevated in sites near to and remote from active myofascial trigger points. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89(1):16–23. PubMed 18164325 ❐

    This significant paper demonstrates that the biochemical milieu of trigger points is acidic and contains many pain-causing metabolites. For much more information about this, see Toxic Muscle Knots.

    (See more detailed commentary on this paper.)

  123. Gerwin RD, Dommerholt J, Shah JP. An expansion of Simons' integrated hypothesis of trigger point formation. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2004 Dec;8(6):468–75. PubMed 15509461 ❐

    Until further notice, the most popular provisional explanation for the trigger point phenomenon is the “expanded integrated hypothesis.” It was first presented in this 2004 paper by Drs. Robert Gerwin, Jan Dommerholt, and Jay Shaw. It’s harrowingly detailed and technical. (Read it just below, if you dare!) When abridged and oversimplified, the “expanded” part is largely lost — it was mostly just filling in some details missing from the original integrated hypothesis (“a possible explanation”), which was put forward by Travell and Simons in the second edition of the Big Red Books in 1999, which was in turn an elaboration on the energy crisis hypothesis that debuted in the first edition in 1981. This has been a work-in-progress for quite a while.

    Here’s a careful translation of the expanded integrated hypothesis:

    Under some circumstances, muscular stresses can cause patches of poor circulation, which results in the pooling of noxious metabolic wastes and high acidity in small areas of the muscle. This is both directly uncomfortable, but also causes a section of the muscle to tighten up and perpetuate a vicious cycle. This predicament is often called an “energy crisis.” It constitutes a subtle lesion. TrPs research has largely been concerned with looking for evidence of a lesion like this.

    And here’s the integrated hypothesis fully spelled out. Brace yourself!

    It can be hypothesized that the activating event in the development of the TrP is the performance of unaccustomed eccentric exercise, eccentric exercise in unconditioned muscle, or maximal or submaximal concentric exercise that leads to muscle fiber damage and to segmental hypercontraction within the muscle fiber. Adding to the physical stress of such exercise is hypoperfusion of the muscle caused by capillary constriction, which results from muscle contraction. Capillary constriction is increased by sympathetic nervous system adrenergic activity. The resultant ischemia and hypoxia adds to the development of tissue injury and produces a local acidic pH with an excess of protons. Acidic pH results in inhibition of acetylcholinesterase activity, increased release of CGRP, and activation of ASIC on muscle nociceptors. Acidic pH alone (in the absence of muscle damage) is sufficient to cause widespread changes in the pain matrix. However, the breakdown of muscle fibers results in the release of several proinflammatory mediators such as SP, CGRP, K+, 5-HT, cytokines, and BK that profoundly alter the activity of the motor endplate and activity/sensitivity of muscle nociceptors and wide dynamic-range neurons. Motor endplate activity is increased because of an apparent increase in the activity of ACh. This apparent increase in effectiveness is caused by several factors that include an increase in the release of ACh that is mediated by CGRP, presynaptic motor terminal adrenergic receptor activity, and by AChE inhibition caused by CGRPand acidic pH. AChRs are up-regulated through the action of CGRP, creating more docking sites for ACh, thereby increasing the efficiency of binding to the receptor. The taut band results from the increase in ACh activity. Miniature endplate potential frequency is increased as a result of greater ACh effect. Release of BK, K+, H+, and cytokines from injured muscle activates the muscle nociceptor receptors, thereby causing tenderness and pain. The presence of CGRP drives the system to become chronic, potentiating the motor endplate response and potentiating, with SP, activation of muscle nociceptors. The combination of acidic pH and proinflammatory mediators at the active TrP contributes to segmental spread of nociceptive input into the dorsal horn of the spinal cord and leads to the activation of multiple receptive fields. Neuroplastic changes in dorsal horn neurons occur in response to continuous nociceptive barrage, causing further activation of neighboring and regional dorsal horn neurons that now have lower thresholds. This results in the observed phenomena of hypersensitivity, allodynia, and referred pain that is characteristic of the active myofascial TrP.

  124. Dommerholt J, Gerwin RD. A Critical Evaluation of Quintner et al: Missing the Point. J Bodyw Mov Ther. 2015 Apr;19(2):193–204. PubMed 25892372 ❐ In this response to criticism, Dommerholt and Gerwin admit that we still don’t really know what’s going on, with statements like “A distinct mechanistic understanding of this disorder does not yet exist” and “ … there has never been a credible anatomic pathology associated with myofascial TrPs” and several more. They don’t seem to mind the uncertainty. They know they’re working with “just a theory.”
  125. Ingraham. Trigger Point Doubts: Do muscle knots exist? Exploring controversies about the existence and nature of so-called “trigger points” and myofascial pain syndrome.  ❐ PainScience.com. 16305 words.
  126. Sarno’s 1999 summary of TMS goes like this: “To summarize, most neck, shoulder and back pain is due to TMS [same as MPS — PI], a harmless physical disorder of the muscles and nerves that is most immediately due to reduced blood circulation to these tissues. This circulatory abnormality results from constriction of the small blood vessels that feed the involved tissues ....” (Sarno)
  127. Gunn CC. Neuropathic Myofascial Pain Syndromes. 3rd ed. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2001.

    The function and integrity of all innervated structures are contingent on the flow of nerve impulses in the intact nerve to provide a regulatory or ‘trophic’ effect. When this flow (probably a combination of axoplasmic flow and electrical input) is blocked, innervated structures are deprived of the trophic factor which is necessary for the control and maintenance of cellular function. ‘A-trophic’ structures become highly irritable and develop abnormal sensitivity or supersensitivity according to Cannon and Rosenblueth’s Law of Denervation ... The importance of disuse supersensitivity cannot be overemphasized. When a nerve malfunctions, the structures it supplies become supersensitive and will behave abnormally. These structures over-react to many forms of input, not only chemical, but physical inputs as well, including stretch and pressure.

  128. We know that these mechanisms probably exist, but there is almost no specific evidence that they have anything to do with the subjective experience of “muscle pain.” Quintner et al. are mainly just proposing to look somewhere other than the integrated hypothesis.
  129. Gerwin 2004, op. cit. Why the EIH? It’s more all-encompassing, more thoroughly considered and studied, has more direct support, and fewer puzzling loose ends than any other hypothesis. There are more serious problems with all the competing hypotheses. Travell and Simons and those that continued their work have simply been at it for longer and did more and better homework. Knowing something about about Travell and Simons’ work is still crucial for any serious manual therapist.
  130. Some possibilities:

    • Maybe our civilization’s sedentariness, postural habits, and routinely cruddy ergonomics predispose us to it. I don’t really buy it — lots of evidence points away from this — but I can’t deny that it as a possibility.
    • Although the spine is not particularly fragile, it may nevertheless be over-protected in various ways by the nervous system because it houses the spinal column.
    • The back may suffer from an excess of trivial pain because of the strain of bipedality (or perhaps only in combination with poor fitness; it’s a perpetually controversial topic).
    • The back may be a psychologically convenient place to “store” stress (I’ll get deeper into this idea below).
    • It may be just an accident of anatomy. Maybe relatively large patches of muscle can be deprived of adequate circulation by stress-induced vasoconstriction of a few blood vessels — there are other “design problems” like this in the human body. (This is straight out of Sarno.)

    Or maybe a little of everything!

  131. Bewyer DC, Bewyer KJ. Rationale for treatment of hip abductor pain syndrome. Iowa Orthop J. 2003;23:57–60. PubMed 14575251 ❐
  132. This assumes that trigger points are in fact a distinct pathological phenomenon — an assumption I just made official for the duration of the book. But even running with that assumption, it remains possible that trigger points are fundamentally neuropathic in character, a distinct sub-type of neuropathy. And so it’s also possible that tps and neuropathy diagnoses get confused because they are essentially the same thing! It’s a bit head twisty, I know. That's why this is a footnote.
  133. In the sense that it is extremely unlikely to be directly, mechanically irritating a nerve in a vulnerable location. A common example is a trigger point in the origin of the gluteus maximus muscle, at the anterior margin, just slightly below and to the right of the PSIS, a prominent bump of bone in the low back. This is a patch of pure muscle with nothing but hip bone underneath it. The sciatic nerve is at least five centimetres away on the surface, and two or three deep. The cluneal nerves are nearby, but can probably only be bothered by pinching them directly onto the bone of the pelvis (iliac crest). Although cluneal nerve entrapment at those locations can cause sensitivity in the glutes, you probably can’t do much to annoy a cluneal nerve by pressing on gluteal muscle. And yet pressure here with the tip of a baby finger can often reproduce the symptoms of “sciatica”: vivid, spreading, shooting pain through the buttock and upper hamstrings (and sometimes further).
  134. Davies C. The trigger point therapy workbook: your self-treatment guide for pain relief. 1st ed. New Harbinger Publications; 2001. p. 24.
  135. For a detailed example from my own experience with injury, see Muscle Pain as an Injury Complication.
  136. Coderre TJ, Katz J, Vaccarino AL, Melzack R. Contribution of central neuroplasticity to pathological pain: review of clinical and experimental evidence. Pain. 1993 Mar;52(3):259–85. PubMed 7681556 ❐

    From the abstract: “Peripheral tissue damage or nerve injury often leads to pathological pain processes, such as spontaneous pain, hyperalgesia and allodynia, that persist for years or decades after all possible tissue healing has occurred.”

  137. Humans have a tendency to start believing their own lies, and when you believe you’re damaged, it’s hard to believe that we don’t start to feel damaged as well. But whether or not that is actually a thing that happens remains surprisingly unknown and controversial.
  138. Maté G. When the Body Says No: The Cost of Hidden Stress. Alfred A. Knopf Canada; 2003.
  139. van der Kolk BA. The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma. Penguin Books; 2014.
  140. Katz J, Rosenbloom BN, Fashler S. Chronic Pain, Psychopathology, and DSM-5 Somatic Symptom Disorder. Can J Psychiatry. 2015 Apr;60(4):160–7. PubMed 26174215 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52584 ❐ As summarized by Vlaeyen in reference to back pain: “The fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM­IV) included a pain­ specific mental disorder, but this diagnosis was not retained in DSM­5 owing to the uncertain importance of medically unexplained pain and the lack of clarity about what psychological factors are of relevance in explaining the symptoms, among other reasons.”
  141. The history of the biology of stress is much like that of research into soft-tissue pain — a handful of determined doctors have worked to educate their colleagues and the public. They have tried to teach us that stress, obvious and otherwise, can cause and significantly complicate pain and illness. For instance: Dr. Hans Selye, a Canadian who virtually invented the study of stress, has written some 30 books and more than 1,500 articles on stress and related problems, including Stress without Distress (1974) and The Stress of Life (1956). Dr. Selye died in 1982 in Montreal, where he had spent fifty years studying the causes and consequences of stress. Another notable example is Dr. Dean Ornish (see Dr. Ornish’s Lifestyle Program), whose ideas about the connection between heart disease and stress were once considered heretical but are now mainstream. There are many more contemporary examples, including both Sarno and Maté, already cited here. Unfortunately, their efforts have largely been ineffective, and to this day … “Most doctors are uncomfortable with medical conditions that have a psychological basis.” (Sarno).
  142. Here’s the rationale for the connection between stress and MPS, jargon included. It’s plausible that emotional stress can cause and sustain any or all of at least three of the pathophysiological processes hypothesized as causes of soft-tissue pain:

    1. stress may directly result in brain-mediated vasoconstriction of arterioles in the postural musculature, resulting in chronic ischemia (Sarno’s hypothesis)
    2. stress may directly result in brain-mediated hyperexcitability of motor neurons, or indirectly by muscular guarding and armouring, causing chronic muscle spasm and tissue fluid stagnancy (Travell/Simons)
    3. stress may cause reflex loop stagnancy indirectly via muscular guarding and armouring. In other words, stress makes you clench your blood vessels and muscles, and/or makes you hold yourself tighter and more defensively, resulting in either overexcited or dulled nerve stimulation.

    This is all speculation. I can’t emphasize strongly enough that no one really knows.

  143. Croft PR, Papageorgiou AC, et al. Psychologic distress and low back pain: Evidence from a prospective study in the general population. Spine. 1995 Dec 15;20(24):2731–7. PubMed 8747252 ❐

    From the abstract: “Symptoms of psychologic distress in individuals without back pain predict the subsequent onset of new episodes of low back pain.”

  144. Waters SJ, Keefe FJ, Strauman TJ. Self-discrepancy in chronic low back pain: relation to pain, depression, and psychological distress. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2004 Mar;27(3):251–9. PubMed 15010103 ❐

    From the abstract, “Results showed that self-discrepancies [stress] can be reliably assessed in patients with persistent [back] pain.”

  145. Carragee EJ, Chen Y, Tanner CM, et al. Provocative discography in patients after limited lumbar discectomy: A controlled and randomized study of pain response in symptomatic and asymptomatic subjects. Spine. 2000 Dec 1;25(23):3065–71. PubMed 1114581 ❐ This was a complex study of a diagnostic procedure. The authors made a somewhat tangential observation: “Patients with abnormal psychological profiles [i.e. people who are under a lot of stress and not coping well] have significantly higher rates of positive disc injections than either asymptomatic volunteers or symptomatic subjects with normal psychological screening.”
  146. Holmes TH, Wolff HG. Life Situations, Emotions and Backache. Psychosom Med. 1952;14(1):18–33.

    Sarno writes of this paper: “Subsequently I found another paper, written by Drs. Holmes and Wolff, both well-known pioneers in the study of pain, that related life situations, emotions and backaches. From this paper came the idea that reduced blood circulation might be the physical basis for [low back pain].”

  147. Sapolsky RM. Why Zebras Don’t Get Ulcers. 3rd ed ed. New York: Times Books; 2004.
  148. No, stress is not your friend. This is the big idea of an extremely popular 2013 TED talk by Kelly McGonigal, and the book that inevitably followed it. Supposedly you are insulated from the health consequences of stress if you just reframe it as a healthy response to a challenge. It’s nonsense on many levels, basically extravagant overinterpretation of a research artifact, as many critics have explained, but “a lie gets halfway around the world before truth puts on its boots.” McGonigal’s talk has become one of the classic examples of the empty sensationalism of so many TED talks.
  149. More from Dr. Sapolsky on this personality type:

    These are not people who are dealing with their stressors too passively, too persistently, too vigilantly, or with too much hostility. They don’t appear to have all that many stressors. They claim they’re not depressed or anxious, and the psychological tests they are given show they’re right. In fact, they describe themselves as pretty happy, successful, and accomplished (and, according to personality tests, they really are). Yet, these people (comprising approximately 5 percent of the population) have chronically activated stress-responses. What’s their problem? Their problem, I think, is one that offers insight into an unexpected vulnerability of our human psyche. The people in question are said to have “repressive” personalities, and we all have met someone like them.

  150. Maté G. When the Body Says No: The Cost of Hidden Stress. Alfred A. Knopf Canada; 2003. p. 28–9.

    Medical thinking usually sees stress as highly disturbing but isolated events such as, for example, sudden unemployment, a marriage breakup, or the death of a loved one. These major events are potent sources of stress for many, but there are chronic daily stresses in people’s lives that are more insidious and more harmful in their long-term biological consequences. Internally generated stresses take their toll without in any way seeming out of the ordinary.

    … While nervous tension may be a component of stress, one can be stressed without feeling tension.

  151. Pérez-Aisa MA, Del Pino D, Siles M, Lanas A. Clinical trends in ulcer diagnosis in a population with high prevalence of Helicobacter pylori infection. Aliment Pharmacol Ther. 2005 Jan;21(1):65–72. PubMed 15644047 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 57160 ❐

    From the article: “This study agrees with previous reports that suggest that the prevalence of peptic ulcer disease has decreased significantly over the last decade in western countries … . The reasons for this decline are not clear … .”

    Of course, the idea that ulcers are in decline because they have become a less psychologically fashionable/acceptable expression of nervous tension is pretty far out in left field. I do think it’s plausible enough to mention here, and potentially consistent with some well-documented strangeness in other conditions. Both carpal tunnel syndrome and the common cold, for instance, have been shown to have frankly baffling epidemiological characteristics, strongly suggesting that they are affected by psychosocial factors beyond our current comprehension. Most doctors would dismiss all of this, though, and agree with the Pérez-Aisa et al. that “a decrease in the prevalence of Helicobacter pylori infection might be the main reason” for the decrease in ulcers.
  152. Weren’t ulcers proved to be caused by a bacterium? That they were. Helicobacter pylori was famously hunted down in 1983 by Australian scientists Barry Marshall and Robin Warren. Although its link with ulceration was initially met with much skepticism, science came around relatively quickly — convinced by evidence, just like it’s supposed to work. By the mid-90s it was widely accepted that H. pylori infection causes ulcers, and Marshall and Warren got a Nobel prize in 2005 (acceptance speech).

    But! Most people infected with the bacterium have no symptoms, and there are many variables that determine the severity of the infection and whether or not it leads to ulcer. Stress is one of those factors (see Guo et al. and Jia et al.). Thus ulcer is very likely both an H. pylori infection and a “stress-sensitive” condition.

  153. Carlesso LC, MacDermid JC, Santaguida PL, Thabane L. A survey of patient's perceptions of what is "adverse" in manual physiotherapy and predicting who is likely to say so. J Clin Epidemiol. 2013 Oct;66(10):1184–91. PubMed 23856189 ❐ Here’s a bit of evidence that back pain makes people nervous and leads to more back pain: in this Canadian survey, low back pain patients were much more likely (51%) to report some kind of unpleasant reaction to therapy than patients with a problem anywhere else in the body, suggesting that back pain makes people nervous. (And/or that actual harm from therapy is more common. But I’m betting on “nervous.”) Also, back pain patients who expected to be “sore” after therapy were somewhat less likely (8.5%) to report a serious reaction.
  154. The idea that some clinical approaches to back pain are actually making back pain worse seems to emerge inevitably from the circumstantial evidence. Bear in mind that iatrogenic illness (illness caused by physicians) is not a new or unusual concept. In 2000, an American presidential task force labelled medical errors a “national problem of epidemic proportions.” The stats need perspective (see Quack Word #20), but they are real. And it’s a short leap from those stats to the research showing that elaborate diagnostic imaging is over-prescribed by doctors who own MRI facilities (Swedlow) and that back pain surgeries are much more common wherever there are more surgeons — not because they are answering a need, mind you, but because they are creating it. Medical care in the UK is just as technological as it is in the United States, but they do a lot fewer back surgeries in the UK. Why? It’s probably not because they are primitive …
  155. Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: Implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2010 Oct;152(2 Suppl):S2–15. PubMed 20961685 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54851 ❐

    Pain itself often modifies the way the central nervous system works, so that a patient actually becomes more sensitive and gets more pain with less provocation. That sensitization is called “central sensitization” because it involves changes in the central nervous system (CNS) in particular — the brain and the spinal cord. Victims are not only more sensitive to things that should hurt, but also to ordinary touch and pressure as well. Their pain also “echoes,” fading more slowly than in other people.

    For a much more detailed summary of this paper, see Sensitization in Chronic Pain.

  156. TheConversation.com [Internet]. Moseley L. Pain really is in the mind, but not in the way you think; 2013 Jul 20 [cited 14 Jan 6]. PainSci Bibliography 54649 ❐

    In this not-too-technical article, the endlessly quotable Lorimer Moseley summarizes the role of the mind in chronic pain, especially low back pain.

  157. Pengel LHM, Herbert RD, Maher CG, Refshauge KM. Acute low back pain: systematic review of its prognosis. BMJ. 2003 Aug;327(7410):323. PubMed 12907487 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51326 ❐

    Many studies over the years that have shown roughly the same thing: most people recover relatively quickly and well. This old review of 15 such studies calculated an average of 58% reduction both pain and disability within a month, and then some more within another month.

  158. Costa LCM, Maher CG, McAuley JH, et al. Prognosis for patients with chronic low back pain: inception cohort study. BMJ. 2009 Oct;339:b3829. PubMed 19808766 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55422 ❐
  159. Costa LCM, Maher CG, Hancock MJ, et al. The prognosis of acute and persistent low-back pain: a meta-analysis. CMAJ. 2012 Aug;184(11):E613–24. PubMed 22586331 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51296 ❐
  160. 51 was the pain level when these patients joined a study, but they had already been in pain for a while. We can guess that it was probably higher at the beginning.
  161. May S, Runge N, Aina A. Centralization and directional preference: An updated systematic review with synthesis of previous evidence. Musculoskelet Sci Pract. 2018 Dec;38:53–62. PubMed 30273918 ❐
  162. Kilpikoski S, Suominen EN, Repo JP, et al. Comparison of magnetic resonance imaging findings among sciatica patients classified as centralizers or non-centralizers. J Man Manip Ther. 2023 Feb:1–10. PubMed 36756675 ❐ This study showed that centralization occurs in somewhat milder cases, while non-centralizers have a bit more pain/disability. It’s not a dramatic difference, but it’s consistent with the rather obvious idea that it’s easier to find relieving movements and positions in less severe cases.
  163. The Keele STarT Back Screening Tool was developed by Keele University, and originally validated in a trial published in The Lancet in 2008 (Hill 2008). Hill went on to study the cost-effectiveness of care customized for patients with low, medium, or high risk of poor outcomes (Hill 2011) — basically, more and better care for people with worse back pain, rather than “one size fits all.” Low-risk patients were reassured and not treated, while medium-risk patients received typical physical therapy, and high-risk patients got more and better physical therapy that was “psychologically informed” with attention to “psychosocial obstacles to recovery.” This approach resulted in better bang-for-buck results. Care stratification has been extensively studied since then, and even extended to other kinds of pain (see van den Broek).
  164. Although the Keele calculator tallies up your score, it doesn’t interpret it for you, and half the scoring table is devoted to highly cryptic codes for each result, thinks like “SNOMED codes, Concept ID: 945621000000107, Description ID: 2418721000000118.” Really, Keele?
  165. Stubbs
  166. Cremers T, Zoulfi Khatiri M, van Maren K, et al. Moderators and Mediators of Activity Intolerance Related to Pain. J Bone Joint Surg Am. 2021 Feb;103(3):205–212. PubMed 33186001 ❐ This is a better-than-nothing citation. Cremers *et al.* over-interpret their moderator/mediator analysis of cross-sectional data, overconfidently “concluding” that fearful misconceptions increases pain-related disability, and more so with greater symptoms of depression or anxiety. That conclusion is actually more like a reasonable hypothesis, partially supported by their experiment. But at least the data doesn’t seem to *undermine* their hypothesis
  167. Sarno J. Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain. Trade paperback, red/blue cover ed. Berkley Books; 1999. p. 27–8.

    Reaching for a traumatic explanation for back pain can become quite absurd. When a minor trauma seems to be the cause of back pain, MPS is inevitably the more significant problem, or even the cause of the incident in the first place. Sarno wrote:

    In 1978 I surveyed a group of a hundred TMS [same as MPS — PI] patients with regard to how their pain started .... Sixty percent reported that when the pain began it was not associated with a physical incident ... However, all of those patients tried to recall something physical that had happened, sometimes going back twenty or thirty years, because they thought there had to be such an incident, since everything they had heard about back pain related it to an injury and a structural problem. It was clear from their histories that there could be no possible relationship between those remote physical incidents and the onset of pain.

  168. Stretch of the erector spinae muscle group, the big columns of muscle on either side of the spine, are limited by the bony and ligamentous structure of the spine: you can only arch your back so far. The gluteus maximus is surprisingly long and lanky, and you can only flex your hip so far before your thigh hits your belly. And so on. These examples and several more are discussed further in the general stretching chapter.
  169. Ch W, Holzman C, Magnusson SP. Increasing muscle extensibility: a matter of increasing length or modifying sensation? Phys Ther. 2010 Mar;90(3):438–49. PubMed 20075147 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55283 ❐
  170. In particular, the big columns of muscle on either side of the spine should feel quite smooth, and the top edge of the gluteus maximus is a long, well-defined diagonal line show across the top of the buttocks.
  171. Acetaminophen (Tylenol) seems to produce mixed results which do not indicate much one way or the other. Muscle relaxants do not work all that well, so their success or failure is not clearly informative (this will be discussed in detail below). The psychoactive drugs and narcotics (anti-depressants, alcohol, marijuana, amphetamines, opioids, benzodiazepines) often seem to help, but the emphasis is on seem, because they mainly affect perception and mood.
  172. Any kind of sensory boat rocking seems to have the potential to do this. I suspect that the shift reflects a change in the brain’s assessment of what tissue concern is the most worrisome, a change in pain priority. It’s as though the body is trying to say, “Now that you’ve scratched the itch on the right, could you please scratch it on the left?” This phenomenon seems to occur much more with the pain that is close to the centre of the body. For instance, I’ve never encountered a case of pain in the forearm switching to the other arm after some treatment — but it’s almost routine with back and neck pain.
  173. Sarno J. Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain. Trade paperback, red/blue cover ed. Berkley Books; 1999. p. 58.

    I did a survey of a large group of patients with TMS [same as MPS — PI] and found that 88 percent had a history of one or more of these conditions [psychophysiological disorders such as heartburn, ulcer, gastritis, hiatus hernia, colitis, spastic colon, migraine, hives, eczema, hay fever, asthma]; 28% had experienced four or more during their lives ...

  174. Heat is reassuring, and comfort is a universal analgesic — it will take the edge off most kinds of pain. Once again the mind game is particularly relevant for back pain: because back pain is so often complicated by anxiety, the soothing power of heat seems to work particularly well on low back muscle pain.
  175. Definitely not always. Temperature sensitivity is variable in fibromyalgia patients. Some are cold sensitive! Other are sensitive to either extreme. But heat-sensitivity is a strong theme.
  176. Morning pain and stiffness is a symptom of several common conditions, including fibromyalgia, osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, plantar fasciitis, Achilles tendinitis, and carpal tunnel syndrome. (Most repetitive strain injuries are probably worse in the morning, but it’s more obvious with some than others.) Most of these do not primarily affect the back, however.
  177. Steffens D, Ferreira ML, Latimer J, et al. What triggers an episode of acute low back pain? A case-crossover study. Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken). 2015 Mar;67(3):403–10. PubMed 25665074 ❐

    This study of triggers for episodes of back pain “brief exposure to a range of physical and psychosocial factors,” mainly being distracted during an activity and awkward postures, can “considerably increase the risk of an episode of acute back pain,” and much more so in the morning:

    Mornings were the most frequent time of day for back pain onset, with 35.2% of participants (n=352) reporting pain onset between 7:00 AM and 10:00 AM (Figure 2). Only 3.7% of participants (n=37) reported pain onset between mid- night and 5:00 AM, with a large increase in reports from 6:00 AM.

  178. IBP and spondyloarthritis are umbrella concepts for several other rheumatic (inflammatory) diseases, including ankylosing spondylitis, reactive arthritis, psoriatic arthritis, inflammatory bowel disease-related arthritis. There are also cases that can’t be classified: “generic” spondyloarthritis. Although it develops slowly, this is one of the rare scary causes of back pain. You don’t want this diagnosis.
  179. Arnbak B, Hendricks O, Hørslev-Petersen K, et al. The discriminative value of inflammatory back pain in patients with persistent low back pain. Scand J Rheumatol. 2016 Mar:1–8. PubMed 26982485 ❐ 82% of more than 700 patients with chronic low back pain reported “morning stiffness” — practically everyone! — but only 11% actually had spondyloarthritis (according to ASAS criteria). Only 10% felt that their pain was worst in the morning. Although these and other inflammatory back pain signs were significantly associated with spondyloarthritis, there’s just too much overlap with other kinds of back pain for morning symptoms to actually identify spondyloarthritis.
  180. If indicated, a doctor can get you checked out for other signs, like blood tests that show elevated CRP, and/or the presence of a particular gene, HLA-B27. An MRI can also help diagnose this kind of inflammation.

  181. Ablin JN, Eshed I, Berman M, et al. Prevalence of axial spondyloarthropathy among patients suffering from Fibromyalgia - an MRI study with application of the ASAS classification criteria. Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken). 2016 Jul. PubMed 27390225 ❐
  182. www.rheumatologynetwork.com [Internet]. Garg N. New and Modified Fibromyalgia Diagnostic Criteria; 2016 August 24 [cited 20 Jul 24]. PainSci Bibliography 53279 ❐

    An excellent explanation (for professionals) of the changes in diagnostic criteria for fibromyalgia. See also Wolfe).

  183. Prolonged chronic stress can probably contribute to metabolic syndrome (Gohil et al) by messing with the hormonal balance of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA-axis).
  184. Franceschi C, Campisi J. Chronic inflammation (inflammaging) and its potential contribution to age-associated diseases. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014 Jun;69 Suppl 1:S4–9. PubMed 24833586 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53291 ❐ “Human aging is characterized by a chronic, low-grade inflammation, and this phenomenon has been termed as "inflammaging." Inflammaging is a highly significant risk factor for both morbidity and mortality in elderly people, as most if not all age-related diseases share an inflammatory pathogenesis. Nevertheless, the precise etiology of inflammaging and its potential causal role in contributing to adverse health outcomes remain largely unknown.”
  185. Hand LE, Hopwood TW, Dickson SH, et al. The circadian clock regulates inflammatory arthritis. FASEB J. 2016 Aug. PubMed 27488122 ❐ This research describes a protein, cryptochrome, used by the body’s “biological clock” to represses inflammation during the night, strongly suggesting that inflammation probably varies in a daily rhythm controlled by the brain. “The clinical implications are far-reaching,” said Thoru Pederson, Ph.D., Editor-in-Chief of The FASEB Journal.
  186. Mäntyselkä P, Kautiainen H, Vanhala M. Prevalence of neck pain in subjects with metabolic syndrome—a cross-sectional population-based study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2010;11:171. PubMed 20670458 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53456 ❐ This study found that neck pain is prevalent in people with metabolic syndrome. The relationship is definitely not necessarily causal, but it certainly might be. This evidence certainly suggests a need for more research to find out.
  187. Stubbs B, Vancampfort D, Thompson T, et al. Pain and severe sleep disturbance in the general population: Primary data and meta-analysis from 240,820 people across 45 low- and middle-income countries. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2018 May;53:52–58. PubMed 29807277 ❐

    This 2018 study went looked at links between pain and severe sleep problems around the world, finding that “pain and sleep problems are highly co-morbid,” echoing what many other studies have found, but specifically confirming the link in diverse populations. They extracted their findings from data on almost a quarter million people in low- and middle-income countries. In other words, there is a strong link between poor sleep and health that is definitely not just a case of the “worried well” sweating the little stuff in their relatively cushy lives. The sleep-health link matters no matter where you’re from.

  188. Gerhart JI, Burns JW, Post KM, et al. Relationships Between Sleep Quality and Pain-Related Factors for People with Chronic Low Back Pain: Tests of Reciprocal and Time of Day Effects. Ann Behav Med. 2017 Jun;51(3):365–375. PubMed 27844327 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51845 ❐
  189. Rohleder N, Aringer M, Boentert M. Role of interleukin-6 in stress, sleep, and fatigue. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012 Jul;1261:88–96. PubMed 22823398 ❐

    The most pertinent passage from this wide-ranging review:

    In addition to the well-documented consequences of low-grade inflammation on the cardiovascular system, for example, the literature summarized here further shows that stress-induced IL-6 increases are closely linked to fatigue and reduced sleep quality. Since sleep is important also for recovery from psychological stress, long-term or repeated stress-induced activation of inflammation is a maladaptive response.

    For inflammatory conditions in particular, these findings add important insights to the well-understood effects of anemia and nighttime pain through inflammation, which is also mediated by IL-6. Evidence clearly suggests that changes in circulating IL-6 can cause changes in sleep quality. Conversely, reductions in sleep duration, quality, or efficiency are capable of increasing peripheral IL-6 concentrations. These results are probably related to the fact that IL-6 increase can also be observed in chronic stress, as a consequence of long-term changes in stress system activity. Taken together, these findings, from various fields of research, underscore the close relationship between IL-6 signaling with CNS processes, thereby making IL-6 a promising candidate for linking adverse CNS states with physical disease.

  190. Manson JE, Patsy M B, Rosen CJ, Taylor CL. Vitamin D Deficiency — Is There Really a Pandemic? N Engl J Med. 2016 Nov 10;375(19):1817–1820. PubMed 27959647 ❐

    ABSTRACT


    The claim that large proportions of North American and other populations are deficient in vitamin D is based on misinterpretation and misapplication of the Institute of Medicine reference values for nutrients — misunderstandings that can adversely affect patient care.

  191. Holick MF, Chen TC. Vitamin D deficiency: a worldwide problem with health consequences. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008 Apr;87(4):1080S–6S. PubMed 18400738 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55028 ❐
  192. Steffens 2015, op. cit.
  193. There is a distinction between “discomfort” and having significant back pain. Sitting a lot at work is not linked to back pain (see Lis, Bakker, Chen). The discomfort we feel when we sit too still for too long time does not seem to be a risk factor for serious episodes of back pain.
  194. There’s no direct evidence for this. It’s a claim that has often been made by clinicians and experts on the topic. Although no one knows exactly what makes trigger points flare up (or go away), extremes of activity and stimulation — too much and too little — are plausible possibilities that seem to be consistent with what patients often report.
  195. I ripped up my shoulder joint playing ultimate (a Frisbee sport). It was hard to sleep for most of a year. When I did sleep, I routinely rolled onto the injured shoulder. Sometimes it woke me up; if it didn’t, I’d wake up with my shoulder howling. And then it would calm down and be mostly fine for the rest of the day. I knew exactly why I had morning shoulder pain, but what if my shoulder was vulnerable for some less obvious reason? I might have thought I had unexplained “morning shoulder pain.”
  196. Matsumura Y, Kasai Y, Obata H, et al. Changes in water content of intervertebral discs and paravertebral muscles before and after bed rest. J Orthop Sci. 2009 Jan;14(1):45–50. PubMed 19214687 ❐
  197. Belavý DL, Miokovic T, Armbrecht G, Felsenberg D. Hypertrophy in the cervical muscles and thoracic discs in bed rest? J Appl Physiol (1985). 2013 Sep;115(5):586–96. PubMed 23813530 ❐
  198. Belavy DL, Adams M, Brisby H, et al. Disc herniations in astronauts: What causes them, and what does it tell us about herniation on earth? Eur Spine J. 2016 Jan;25(1):144–54. PubMed 25893331 ❐ “The most likely cause for lumbar IVD herniations was concluded to be swelling of the IVD in the unloaded condition during spaceflight.”
  199. Annular tears or basically arthritic fissures in the disc — splitting like chapped lips. “Annular” because the lesion is in the annulus fibrosis, or outer layer of the disc.
  200. Nederhand MJ, Hermens HJ, Ijzerman MJ, Groothuis KGM, Turk DC. The effect of fear of movement on muscle activation in posttraumatic neck pain disability. Clinical Journal of Pain. 2006 Jul-Aug;22(6):519–525. PubMed 16788337 ❐

    This study of whiplash patients showed that muscle tone is inhibited, not increased, let alone spasmed. “It is likely that the decrease in muscle activation level is aimed at ‘avoiding’ the use of painful muscles.”

  201. Mense S, Simons DG, Russell IJ. Muscle pain: understanding its nature, diagnosis and treatment. 1st hardcover ed. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2000. p. 259.

    The old concept of a pain-spasm-pain cycle does not stand up to experimental verification either from a physiologic point of view or from a clinical point of view.

    Physiologic studies show that muscle pain tends to inhibit, not facilitate, reflex contractile activity of the same muscle ...

    In 1989, Ernest Johnson, editor of the American Journal of Physical Medicine, summarized overwhelming evidence that the common perception of muscle pain being closely related to muscle spasm is a myth and that the myth has been strongly encouraged by commercial interests.

  202. Spasm could be fairly evenly distributed, but probably not so much that it would fool experienced massage therapists. Most back pain is strikingly one-sided, and it’s unlikely that related spasming would be tidily symmetrical.
  203. Belavý DL, Quittner MJ, Ridgers N, et al. Running exercise strengthens the intervertebral disc. Scientific Reports. 2017 Apr;7:45975. PubMed 28422125 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53606 ❐
  204. Chiu 2015, op. cit.
  205. Maigne R. Manipulation of the spine. In Basmajian JV (ed): Manipulation, Traction and Massage, Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. 1986.
  206. Hertling D, Kessler R. Management of common musculoskeletal disorders. 3rd ed. Lippincott; 1996. p574. Darlene Hertling clearly elucidates Maigne’s ideas about MIDs, with reference to the thoracic spine. Likely the idea can be sensibly applied to other sections of the spine as well.
  207. Where is muscle pain? Compared to facet joints, the anatomical location of the source of muscle pain is like trying to find a smoke signal in the fog. We aren’t even sure if there is such a thing as a discrete painful lesion in muscle (other than bruises). Even if there is, they aren’t easy to reliably locate.
  208. Let me explain “wee little nerves” with a bit more jargon for my professional readers: the nerves in question here are the medial branches of the dorsal ramus nerves that innervate the facet joints.
  209. Falco FJE, Manchikanti L, Datta S, et al. Systematic review of the therapeutic effectiveness of cervical facet joint interventions: an update. Pain Physician. 2012;15(6):E839–68. PubMed 23159978 ❐

    This small 2012 review concluded that the “evidence for cervical medial branch blocks is fair.” Unfortunately, “fair” is a ridiculous word to use when summing up a “body” of evidence based on one trial (no matter how good) and one prospective evaluation. They also granted “fair” evidence for radiofrequency neurotomy based on just one randomized controlled trial (and a few almost meaningless observational studies).

    Evidence for two other cervical joint interventions was “limited” by comparison! Indeed.

    In my opinion, they did not find enough of any kind of evidence about anything to draw any conclusions whatsoever.

    This paper is very similar to Manchikanti et al, regarding the thoracic spine (again involving some of the same researchers).

  210. Atluri S, Datta S, Falco FJE, Lee M. Systematic review of diagnostic utility and therapeutic effectiveness of thoracic facet joint interventions. Pain Physician. 2008;11(5):611–29. PubMed 18850026 ❐

    A review of barely adequate scientific literature on thoracic spinal joint interventions, but some of what they found was promising: good (Level I or II-1) for both diagnostic and therapeutic nerve blocks. But the number of papers on this topic really is extremely small: even years later, Manchikanti 2012 only reviewed a handful. The optimistic conclusion here is not resting on much.

  211. Datta S, Lee M, Falco FJE, Bryce DA, Hayek SM. Systematic assessment of diagnostic accuracy and therapeutic utility of lumbar facet joint interventions. Pain Physician. 2009;12(2):437–60. PubMed 19305489 ❐

    This review of diagnosis and treatment of lumbar facet joints found very little, a “paucity of published literature,” but what they did find seemed to be positive: good evidence for diagnostic nerve blocks, and fair when using them for treatment. Notably, Staal concluded that the literature was too scanty to conclude anything.

  212. Wasan AD, Jamison RN, Pham L, et al. Psychopathology predicts the outcome of medial branch blocks with corticosteroid for chronic axial low back or cervical pain: a prospective cohort study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2009;10:22. PubMed 19220916 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55303 ❐ “Psychiatric comorbidity is associated with diminished pain relief after a MBB injection performed with steroid at one-month follow-up.”
  213. Ben Cormack, Peter Stilwell, Sabrina Coninx, Jo Gibson. The biopsychosocial model is lost in translation: from misrepresentation to an enactive modernization. Physiotherapy Theory and Practice. 2022:1–16. PubMed 35645164 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52047 ❐
  214. Moseley GL. I can't find it! Distorted body image and tactile dysfunction in patients with chronic back pain. Pain. 2008 Nov;140(1):239–43. PubMed 18786763 ❐

    In this small, unusual study, six patients with low back pain were asked to draw their perceptions of their back and spine. For comparison, a group of ten patients with no recent back pain were asked to do the same exercise. They were encouraged to “draw what it feels like” rather than how it actually looked.

    “All the patients, and none of the controls, showed disrupted body image of the back” on the same side and level as the pain. For instance, they did not have a clear sense of the outline of their trunk.

    Most intriguingly, patients with back pain on just one side illustrated vertebrae deviated towards the painful side (without any obvious actual deviation).

  215. French SD, Green S, Forbes A. Reliability of chiropractic methods commonly used to detect manipulable lesions in patients with chronic low-back pain. J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2000 May;23(4):231–8. PubMed 10820295 ❐
  216. Goode A, Hegedus EJ, Sizer P, et al. Three-dimensional movements of the sacroiliac joint: a systematic review of the literature and assessment of clinical utility. J Man Manip Ther. 2008;16(1):25–38. PubMed 19119382 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53407 ❐

    This review of seven good quality studies of sacroliac joint movement found that it is “limited to minute amounts of rotation and of translation.” Specifically, there’s a maximum of about 8˚ rotation and 8mm translation, which might sound like more than a “minute” amount, but those are very specific maximums. The “average” motion would be much less, just a couple degrees and millimetres in most cases. According to this data, it’s probably difficult and unreliable for therapists to determine SIJ movement and position by feel.

  217. Sekharappa V, Amritanand R, Krishnan V, David KS. Lumbosacral transition vertebra: prevalence and its significance. Asian Spine Journal. 2014 Feb;8(1):51–8. PubMed 24596605 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54202 ❐
  218. Dar G, Khamis S, Peleg S, et al. Sacroiliac joint fusion and the implications for manual therapy diagnosis and treatment. Man Ther. 2008 May;13(2):155–8. PubMed 17368076 ❐
  219. Lonser RR, Resnick DK. Current Controversies in Spinal and Cranial Surgery. Neurosurg Clin N Am. 2017 07;28(3):xiii–xiv. PubMed 28600018 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52617 ❐
  220. Zaidi HA, Montoure AJ, Dickman CA. Surgical and clinical efficacy of sacroiliac joint fusion: a systematic review of the literature. J Neurosurg Spine. 2015 Jul;23(1):59–66. PubMed 25840040 ❐ "Surgical intervention for SIJ pain is beneficial in a subset of patients. However, with the difficulty in accurate diagnosis and evidence for the efficacy of SIJ fusion itself lacking, serious consideration of the cause of pain and alternative treatments should be given before performing the operation.
  221. Tullberg T, Blomberg S, Branth B, Johnsson R. Manipulation does not alter the position of the sacroiliac joint. A roentgen stereophotogrammetric analysis. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1998 May;23(10):1124–8; discussion 1129. PubMed 9615363 ❐
  222. Dall BE, Eden SV, Cho W, et al. Biomechanical analysis of motion following sacroiliac joint fusion using lateral sacroiliac screws with or without lumbosacral instrumented fusion. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). 2019 May;68:182–189. PubMed 31234032 ❐

    For this experiment, SIJ ligaments in cadavers were severed, roughly doubling the joint’s range of motion. Screwing it back together decreased the instability but did not eliminate it. Even a more elaborate rig of screws and fixation rods could not fully restore the stability of the intact joint. This failure to stabilize big, complex joints is also seen with other kinds of spinal fixation: see Spinal Fracture Bracing and Fixation

    a href="#ref183" class="backref">BACK TO TEXT
  223. Cooperstein R, Hickey M. The reliability of palpating the posterior superior iliac spine: a systematic review. J Can Chiropr Assoc. 2016 Mar;60(1):36–46. PubMed 27069265 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53310 ❐
  224. Goode A, Hegedus EJ, Sizer P, et al. Three-dimensional movements of the sacroiliac joint: a systematic review of the literature and assessment of clinical utility. J Man Manip Ther. 2008;16(1):25–38. PubMed 19119382 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53407 ❐

    This review of seven good quality studies of sacroliac joint movement found that it is “limited to minute amounts of rotation and of translation.” Specifically, there’s a maximum of about 8˚ rotation and 8mm translation, which might sound like more than a “minute” amount, but those are very specific maximums. The “average” motion would be much less, just a couple degrees and millimetres in most cases. According to this data, it’s probably difficult and unreliable for therapists to determine SIJ movement and position by feel.

  225. Specifically, it could be the adjacent “Perfect Spot No.12” — a common and irritating sore spot in the upper reaches of the gluteus maximus. In my clinical experience, many or most low back pain clients with an alleged sacroiliac joint problem have no such problem that I can detect, but they usually do have this sensitive spot in the neighbouring muscle.
  226. Cooperstein R, Lucente M. Comparison of supine and prone methods of leg length inequality assessment. J Chiropr Med. 2017 Jun;16(2):103–110. PubMed 28559750 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52779 ❐

    Assessments of leg length are common, both with the patient lying down or standing. Either could be reliable, but in this test they did not agree with each other. Two chiropractors with more than 30 years experience each assessed the same few dozen patients, and agreement between their results when they felt confident in them was “perfectly nil.“ Despite the widespread and confident use of each method, this test clearly suggests that at least one of them is unreliable, but it’s also entirely possible that both of them are.

  227. Grundy PF, Roberts CJ. Does unequal leg length cause back pain? A case-control study. Lancet. 1984 Aug 4;2(8397):256–8. PubMed 6146810 ❐

    This classic, elegant experiment found no connection between leg length and back pain. Like most of the really good science experiments, it has that MythBusters attitude: “why don’t we just check that assumption?” Researchers measured leg lengths, looking for differences in “lower limb length and other disproportion at or around the sacroiliac joints” and found no association with low back pain. “Chronic back pain is thus unlikely to be part of the short-leg syndrome.” Other studies since have backed this up, but this simple old paper remains a favourite.

  228. Morgenroth DC, Shakir A, Orendurff MS, Czerniecki JM. Low-back pain in transfemoral amputees: is there a correlation with static or dynamic leg-length discrepancy? Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2009 Feb;88(2):108–13. PubMed 19169176 ❐

    “There were no statistically significant differences between the pain and no pain groups in terms … [any kind of] leg length difference.” Amputees are a special population, and (obviously) not necessarily representative of back pain in non-amputees. And yet, if leg length differences mattered to back pain in general, they would probably be more common in the amputees with pain.

  229. Chuter V, Spink M, Searle A, Ho A. The effectiveness of shoe insoles for the prevention and treatment of low back pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2014 Apr;15:140. PubMed 24775807 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53612 ❐

    The evidence available on this topic — so far, such as it is, just a few small trials — suggests that insoles or foot orthoses do not prevent or treat back pain, which is hardly a surprise. Technically the evidence is just inadequate and inconclusive, but the absence of any benefit in the small trials done so far is damning.

  230. Cambron JA, Dexheimer JM, Duarte M, Freels S. Shoe Orthotics for the Treatment of Chronic Low Back Pain: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2017 Apr. PubMed 28465224 ❐

    This trial tested the efficacy of shoe orthotics and chiropractic treatment for chronic low back pain. 225 patients were divided into three groups: a wait-list control group got no care at all, another group received custom, and a third group got custom orthotics plus chiropractic and massage treatment. The authors reported positive results up: “Six weeks of prescription shoe orthotics significantly improved back pain and dysfunction compared to no treatment.” But no differences after 12 weeks! And that’s even with an unfair advantage …

    Wait-list groups are a crappy control. They suffer from “frustrebo” — poor outcomes caused by disappointment of knowing that you are not getting any treatment (see Power). This is a major flaw. As Dr. Stephen Ward tweeted, “Waiting list control, schwaiting list control.

    The lack of a true placebo control is a deal-breaker here, especially put in the context of other studies of orthotics for back pain, which are negative (e.g. see Chuter). Without replication, I think this study’s results can be dismissed as an outlier attributable to a flawed design and a high risk of bias.

  231. Richter RR, Austin TM, Reinking MF. Foot orthoses in lower limb overuse conditions: a systematic review and meta-analysis--critical appraisal and commentary. J Athl Train. 2011;46(1):103–6. PubMed 21214358 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53613 ❐

    A review of studies of foot orthoses to treat overuse injuries of the lower limb with a mostly negative conclusion: “no difference between custom and prefabricated foot orthoses” and “the evidence was insufficient to recommend foot orthoses (custom or prefabricated).” (One thin ray of light was that the evidence supposedly supports the use of foot orthoses to prevent a first injury … but, even if true, hardly anyone seeks out orthoses before they develop an overuse injury. And why would they be good only for a first injury? Makes no sense, therefore likely not true.)

  232. Schmidt H, Bashkuev M, Weerts J, et al. How do we stand? Variations during repeated standing phases of asymptomatic subjects and low back pain patients. J Biomech. 2018 Mar;70:67–76. PubMed 28683929 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53085 ❐

    We rarely stand quite the same way twice in a row — so good luck identifying a poor posture. In a 2018 experiment, researchers measured and re-measured lumbar spinal curvature (lordosis) in hundreds of people using a handy curve-o-meter, and found that it changed from one test to the next to a surprising degree … and continued to do so with repeat tests. Measure a spine five times in a row, get a wide variety of results!

    And there was no difference between spinal position in 350 people versus 80 with with back pain. And there was also no difference between athletes and non-athletes. Age, gender, height, and weight made no difference either. In everyone, standing posture was “highly individual and poorly reproducible.” Which is one major reason why posture assessments is just nonsense. It’s unreliable even using an objective measuring gadget — never mind when you introduce the biased eyeballing of a trainer or massage therapist looking for postural trouble to shoot.

    See MassageFitnessMag.com for a more detailed analysis.

  233. Fahrni WH, Trueman GE. Comparative Radiological Study of the Spines of a Primitive Population With North Americans and Northern Europeans. J Bone Joint Surg Br. 1965 Aug;47:552–5. PubMed 14341078 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53604 ❐
  234. Nourbakhsh MR, Arab AM. Relationship between mechanical factors and incidence of low back pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2002 Sep;32(9):447–60. PubMed 12322811 ❐

    The key findings:

    “It appears that muscle endurance and weakness are associated with LBP and that structural factors such as the size of the lumbar lordosis, pelvic tilt, leg length discrepancy, and the length of abdominal, hamstring, and iliopsoas muscles are not associated with the occurrence of LBP.”

    Nourbakhs and Arab did indeed identify a correlation between cLBP and pain and poor muscle endurance and strength, but correlation is (famously) not causation. They did not establish that the relationship is causal, or that improving these metrics is therapeutic, so note that their data is not very useful for supporting the value of core training as an intervention for back pain. And so, despite finding weak back muscles in back pain patients, it cannot by itself support the value core training (meanwhile, there's a bunch of other more relevant and recent evidence to consider).

    What I think is most significant in their results is that found not even a correlation between chronic low back pain and various "structural" features. And where there is not even a correlation, there cannot be causation! So this data is useful for supporting the point that structure doesn't seem to have much to do with back pain.

  235. Murrie VL, Dixon AK, Hollingworth W, Wilson H, Doyle TAC. Lumbar lordosis: study of patients with and without low back pain. Clin Anat. 2003 Mar;16(2):144–7. PubMed 12589669 ❐

    This study showed that there was no difference in lumbar curvature between 27 people with and 19 without low back pain, and also no difference between people of different ages.

  236. Nakipoğlu GF, Karagöz A, Ozgirgin N. The biomechanics of the lumbosacral region in acute and chronic low back pain patients. Pain Physician. 2008;11(4):505–11. PubMed 18690279 ❐

    This study show no difference in thoracic kyphosis (hunchback curve), lumbar lordosis (low back curve) and sacral inclination between people with acute low back pain and chronic low back pain.

  237. Christensen ST, Hartvigsen J. Spinal curves and health: a systematic critical review of the epidemiological literature dealing with associations between sagittal spinal curves and health. J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2008;31(9):690–714. PubMed 19028253 ❐

    This review of more than 50 studies found no association between measurements of spinal curves and pain. The authors’ conclusion was decisive: the evidence “does not support an association between sagittal spinal curves and health including spinal pain.” One can cherry pick the data for a few studies that show some minor correlation, but it averages out to nothing to write home about.

  238. Certainly this happens. For instance, in cases of clear trauma, the lordotic curve may change quite radically — that’s simply the body adapting to an unpleasant situation, seeking the position of greatest safety.
  239. Whitcome KK, Shapiro LJ, Lieberman DE. Fetal load and the evolution of lumbar lordosis in bipedal hominins. Nature. 2007;450(7172):1075–1078.
  240. Quick refresher course in natural selection. (I think about evolutionary mechanisms relevant to health care every day, but most people don’t, so you might appreciate a quick reminder of how this works!) If you die before you can breed, your genes don’t get passed on to your children, and such features steadily get “bred out” of the species. Biological features that make you more likely to succeed at procreating will get passed on to your children and become more and more common in the species over time. Whitcome’s research suggests that women with weaker spines, over the aeons, often failed to successfully carry their babies to term because the strain was debilitating.
  241. Nourbakhsh MR, Arab AM. Relationship between mechanical factors and incidence of low back pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2002 Sep;32(9):447–60. PubMed 12322811 ❐

    The key findings:

    “It appears that muscle endurance and weakness are associated with LBP and that structural factors such as the size of the lumbar lordosis, pelvic tilt, leg length discrepancy, and the length of abdominal, hamstring, and iliopsoas muscles are not associated with the occurrence of LBP.”

    Nourbakhs and Arab did indeed identify a correlation between cLBP and pain and poor muscle endurance and strength, but correlation is (famously) not causation. They did not establish that the relationship is causal, or that improving these metrics is therapeutic, so note that their data is not very useful for supporting the value of core training as an intervention for back pain. And so, despite finding weak back muscles in back pain patients, it cannot by itself support the value core training (meanwhile, there's a bunch of other more relevant and recent evidence to consider).

    What I think is most significant in their results is that found not even a correlation between chronic low back pain and various "structural" features. And where there is not even a correlation, there cannot be causation! So this data is useful for supporting the point that structure doesn't seem to have much to do with back pain.

  242. Hussain SM, Urquhart DM, Wang Y, et al. Fat mass and fat distribution are associated with low back pain intensity and disability: results from a cohort study. Arthritis Res Ther. 2017 Feb;19(1):26. PubMed 28183360 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52908 ❐
  243. Dario AB, Loureiro Ferreira M, Refshauge K, et al. Obesity does not increase the risk of chronic low back pain when genetics are considered. A prospective study of Spanish adult twins. Spine J. 2017 Feb;17(2):282–290. PubMed 27751965 ❐
  244. From mentors! It was just a pervasive idea in my new profession… which, I was discovering, wasn’t exactly inclined to, you know, look things up. All I really had was the *tidyness* of the claim, its intense common-sensical quality.
  245. Knowledge is ultimately less important than how we acquire it. I often use “the journey from ignorance to knowledge” as a light narrative structure for explaining what I currently believe to be true, implying that “this is probably not over.” It’s just a step in a process.

    And (pro tip) the “story” of how you get to a conclusion is just a bit more interesting, a simple way to “moisten” what would otherwise be a rather dry information snack. It’s a way to have any narrative structure at all, with subject matter that doesn’t lend itself to that.

    This is the story of my long journey to a conclusion that back pain is not a modern disease, but that journey is probably not over, because they never are. I just have a good-enough-for-now, based on much better data than I had before.

  246. Martin BI, Deyo RA, Mirza SK, et al. Expenditures and Health Status Among Adults With Back and Neck Problems. JAMA. 2008;299(6):656–664. PainSci Bibliography 56228 ❐
  247. Jiménez-Sánchez S, Jiménez-García R, Hernández-Barrera V, et al. Has the prevalence of invalidating musculoskeletal pain changed over the last 15 years (1993-2006)? A Spanish population-based survey. J Pain. 2010 Jul;11(7):612–20. PubMed 20356799 ❐
  248. Harkness EF, Macfarlane GJ, Silman AJ, McBeth J. Is musculoskeletal pain more common now than 40 years ago? Two population-based cross-sectional studies. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2005 Jul;44(7):890–5. PubMed 15784630 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54796 ❐
  249. Steele J. An evolutionary hypothesis to explain the role of deconditioning in low back pain prevalence in humans. Journal of Evolution and Health. 2017 01;1.
  250. In more detail: >“A handful of studies do suggest that some traditional cultures report extremely low rates of LBP including some studies of Australian Aborigines and rural Thai. However, in combination with the totality of evidence suggesting that LBP is common to a range of traditional populations similar to these it is reasonable to consider these findings as outliers. Explanation for these outliers concerns mainly the reduced inclination of indigenous peoples to report pain to ‘outsiders’ due to cultural barriers.”

  251. Specifically, that “an evolutionarily determined factor might predispose LBP across a wide range of Homo sapiens populations.” In other words, humans are vulnerable to back pain because we have a bit of an anatomical glitch. This is a point I will almost certainly devote a chapter to in the future.
  252. The idea that prehistoric movement lifestyles were truly ideal is a superfluous, entertaining, and untestable extrapolation from the popular belief that “highly active lifestyles,” wherever they are found, protect people from back pain. We are not so much concerned with the difference between us and prehistoric hunter-gatherers, but between monotonously sedentary lifestyles versus richly active ones — and modern ones will do. And extant traditional cultures clearly have wildly different and more active “movement lifestyles” than the participants in economies based on information, commerce, highly automated industry, etc. The difference between these modern populations should be different enough to produce a major difference in back pain prevalence. And it does not.
  253. O'Sullivan P. It's time for change with the management of non-specific chronic low back pain. Br J Sports Med. 2012 Mar;46(4):224–7. PubMed 21821612 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52124 ❐
  254. In a short article for Body In Mind, Neil O’Connell, a UK low back pain researcher, does a wonderfully curmudgeonly job of summarizing the history of core strengthening:

    Core stability is an interesting case. It divides opinion and yet stands as one of the preeminent models for treating back pain through rehabilitation. In the mid-nineties a rehabilitation movement was created, born from the experimental observation that deep abdominal and paraspinal muscles are altered in their activation patterns in patients with back pain. Those early experiments and subsequent ones gave the model plausibility but, for me, what was more fascinating than the phenomenon of altered trunk muscle function was the phenomenon of a clinical dogma thrusting its way relentlessly through the therapy world. From this fair but limited data, enthusiastic entrepreneurs and self-elected authorities duly sprinted with the ball, creatively developing detailed treatment approaches with strict and specific rules that far exceeded the actual data. “Contract this muscle but not that one, definitely not that one” or “move like this, not like that”, spreading the empirically unsubstantiated (but potentially harmful?) concept to therapists and patients alike of spinal segmental instability, wherein poorly controlled vertebral segments shear excessively resulting in pain. You too could have the answer to treating back pain as long as you attend these 5 sequential courses at $$$$ a pop. Colleagues were falling over each other to buy the equipment needed to apply this model that had been accepted as gospel, from not-too-pricey pressure biofeedback cushions to very-pricey-indeed real time ultrasound imaging devices and the rehabilitation philosophy of the long-late Joseph Pilates experienced a remarkable resurrection. As a case study into how new treatment approaches are adopted in our profession it is perhaps second to none. Maybe the model was correct – we couldn’t know at that time, but in my early clinical career core stability came to dominate thinking as an accepted truth way before we had a good answer about whether it worked.

  255. Macedo LG, Maher CG, Latimer J, McAuley JH. Motor Control Exercise for Persistent, Nonspecific Low Back Pain: A Systematic Review. Phys Ther. 2009 Jan;89(1):ptj.20080103. PubMed 19056854 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 56118 ❐
  256. Bray H, Moseley GL. Disrupted working body schema of the trunk in people with back pain. Br J Sports Med. 2011 Mar;45(3):168–73. PubMed 19887441 ❐

    If someone is suffering from low back pain, is it possible that they are less accurate in making left/right trunk rotation judgements? Apparently so. The researchers concluded: “Chronic back pain is associated with disruption of the working body schema [mental picture] of the trunk. This might be an important contributor to motor control abnormalities seen in this population.”

    But it’s very important to note that the arrow of causation could swing back and forth like a compass in an MRI machine. Is poor coordination causing low back pain? Or is low back causing poor coordination? Or do they just happen to go well together, like peanut butter and chocolate?

  257. Indeed, the observation that patients with low back pain have screwy core stability is the basic fact that launched the entire core fad.
  258. Luomajoki H, Moseley GL. Tactile acuity and lumbopelvic motor control in patients with back pain and healthy controls. Br J Sports Med. 2011 Apr;45(5):437–40. PubMed 19553222 ❐

    This study showed that patients with back pain have decreased tactile acuity and coordination — that is, their sense of touch goes on the blink, and their movement precision is a bit wonky. Touch accuracy is determined by the minimum distance apart that two points of contact can be distinguished: 10mm for a healthy person, but 13mm for those with low back pain. The worse the touch accuracy in a patient, the worse the coordination as well. The authors speculated that “training tactile acuity may aid recovery and assist in achieving normal motor performance after back injury.” That’s quite a leap, but it’s not an unreasonable question to ask (and it could be an example of the potential value of touch therapy for assisting with normalizing a patient’s sense of self).

  259. Csapo R, Maganaris CN, Seynnes OR, Narici MV. On muscle, tendon and high heels. J Exp Biol. 2010 Aug;213(Pt 15):2582–8. PubMed 20639419 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55265 ❐
  260. Kesselheim AS, Mello MM, Studdert DM. Strategies and practices in off-label marketing of pharmaceuticals: a retrospective analysis of whistleblower complaints. PLoS Med. 2011 Apr;8(4):e1000431. PubMed 21483716 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53208 ❐
  261. The chiropractic profession was founded on the so-called “Big Idea” that all illness is caused by spinal joint subluxation which pinches nerve roots and interferes with the flow of nerve signals through nerve roots. Thus the entire profession is essentially a public education engine for the false idea that nerve roots get pinched a lot. For example, see The Straw Protocol.
  262. Torun F, Dolgun H, Tuna H, et al. Morphometric analysis of the roots and neural foramina of the lumbar vertebrae. Surgical Neurology. 2006 Aug;66(2):148–51; discussion 151. PubMed 16876606 ❐ This was exasperatingly hard data to find for some reason, and the paper abstract begins by saying so: “There have been few anatomic studies on the foramina and roots of the lumbar region … .” This is in a 2006 paper! Hardly ancient.
  263. Takasaki H, Hall T, Jull G, et al. The influence of cervical traction, compression, and spurling test on cervical intervertebral foramen size. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009 Jul;34(16):1658–62. PubMed 19770608 ❐
  264. Sari H, Akarirmak U, Karacan I, Akman H. Computed tomographic evaluation of lumbar spinal structures during traction. Physiother Theory Pract. 2005;21(1):3–11. PubMed 16385939 ❐
  265. Ebraheim NA, Liu J, Ramineni SK, et al. Morphological changes in the cervical intervertebral foramen dimensions with unilateral facet joint dislocation. Injury. 2009 Nov;40(11):1157–60. PubMed 19486975 ❐

    Researchers dislocated neck joints in corpses to measure the effect on the size of the intervertebral foramina. (Interesting chore!) Dislocation made the spaces quite a bit larger, indicating that any nerve root pain associated with these injuries “is probably due to distraction rather than due to direct nerve root compression.”

  266. Andrade NS, Ashton CM, Wray NP, Brown C, Bartanusz V. Systematic review of observational studies reveals no association between low back pain and lumbar spondylolysis with or without isthmic spondylolisthesis. Eur Spine J. 2015 Jun;24(6):1289–95. PubMed 25833204 ❐

    It is widely believed that spondylolysis (SL) and/or isthmic spondylolisthesis (IS) cause low back pain. If so, individuals with these conditions should be more prone to back pain (duh). This paper reviewed other studies looking for that association. They found 15 adequate studies. None of them detected an association between SL/IS and LBP. The authors speculate that the two apparent benefits of treatments may just be “due to benign natural history and nonspecific treatment effects.” They suggest that “traditional surgical practice … should be reconsidered.”

  267. Slow- and moderate-intensity compression of most healthy nerves is completely painless, while unhealthy or injured nerves can easily be irritated by light pressure or even brushing of the skin. Of course, if you hit a healthy nerve hard enough, it will zing! Your “funny bone” is the most obvious example: the ulnar nerve at the elbow is all too easy to smash against a backstop of bone! But ordinary pressures on most nerves really does not cause any significant pain, I promise — I press on nerves all the time. So this raises an important question: why would nerves ever hurt if they haven’t actually been traumatized? If an injured nerve hurts, it’s probably because it’s ischemic (oxygen-starved) or irritated by other abnormal tissue chemistry. See Wilson, and Mackinnon, Kobayashi (the next three footnotes) …
  268. Wilson CB. Significance of the small lumbar spinal canal: cauda equina compression syndromes due to spondylosis 3: Intermittent claudication. J Neurosurg. 1969;31:499–506. PubMed 5351760 ❐

    An old topic review that explains that the belief that the pain may be caused by impairment of circulation to the capillaries of the spinal nerve roots.

  269. Mackinnon SE. Pathophysiology of nerve compression. Hand Clin. 2002 May;18(2):231–41. PubMed 1237102 ❐

    From the abstract: “Both ischemic and mechanical factors are involved in the development of compression neuropathy.” In other words, mechanical factors only — just being pinched — probably does not cause nerve pain.

  270. Kobayashi S, Shizu N, Suzuki Y. Changes in nerve root motion and intraradicular blood flow during an intraoperative straight-leg-raising test. Spine. 2003 Jul 1;28(13):1427–34. PubMed 12838102 ❐

    Kobay et al. surgically examined blood flow to a lumbar nerve root while the leg was in a painful position. (They peeked into twelve backs with a history of symptomatic disk herniations and nerve pain.) They found that “the intraoperative reverse straight leg raise test showed that the hernia compressed the nerve roots, and that there was marked disturbance of gliding, which was reduced to only a few millimeters,” and “during the test, intraradicular blood flow showed a sharp decrease [40 to 98%] at the angle that produced sciatica.”

    Intriguing. It’s probably the physical distortion of the nerve root that caused the loss of circulation, and the combination of the two that was painful. Successful treatment seemed to back this up: “After removal of the hernia, all the patients showed smooth gliding of the nerve roots during the second intraoperative test, and there was no marked decrease in intraradicular blood flow.”

  271. Jayson MI. The role of vascular damage and fibrosis in the pathogenesis of nerve root damage. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 1992 Jun;(279):40–8. PubMed 1534723 ❐ It “appears likely that venous obstruction with resultant hypoxia is an important mechanism leading to nerve root damage.”
  272. Costigan M, Belfer I, Griffin RS, et al. Multiple chronic pain states are associated with a common amino acid-changing allele in KCNS1. Brain. 2010 Sep;133(9):2519–27. PubMed 20724292 ❐

    Mark your calendars: 2010 was the year researchers confirmed a gene as “one of the first prognostic indicators of chronic pain risk,” doubling or tripling the odds that a low back pain patient will recover in a timely fashion from nerve root injury. Screening for this gene is not yet something that is clinically available, but it probably will be someday, and then you will know: the universe really does hate you.

  273. Lauder TD. Musculoskeletal disorders that frequently mimic radiculopathy. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. 2002;13(3):469–485. PubMed 12380546 ❐

    From the abstract: “Treatment of concomitant musculoskeletal disorders in patients with radiculopathy improves patient satisfaction and outcomes.”

  274. My therapist readers might reasonably ask: Is it possible that the pressure on the side of the hip was causing a reactive contraction in the piriformis muscle, resulting in an impingement of the sciatic nerve … and fooling me? Yes, this is possible. But I doubt it. This patient did not have a “twitchy” piriformis, as so many people with piriformis syndrome do. She was generally relaxed, optimistic, and not in much pain — already about 50% recovered. I find it unlikely that her piriformis muscle could be so prone to spasm at this stage. Nor could I palpate any reactive spasm in the piriformis during treatment, not even when prodding the piriformis directly. It tolerated stretch well, and was not even particularly high-toned at rest.
  275. Braddom 2009, op. cit. That is, you can demonstrate that their nerve roots aren’t working properly by electrically stimulating them to trigger nerve impulses. If they don’t transmit enough nerve impulses, something’s wrong.
  276. Sekharappa V, Amritanand R, Krishnan V, David KS. Lumbosacral transition vertebra: prevalence and its significance. Asian Spine Journal. 2014 Feb;8(1):51–8. PubMed 24596605 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54202 ❐

    Sometimes the sacrum is fused to the lowest lumbar vertebra: a lumbosacral transition vertebra. “LSTV is the most common congenital anomaly of the lumbosacral spine.” In about a thousand patients studied, it was about twice as common in patients who had sought spinal surgery as it was in patients with no spinal complaint (about 14-16% of patients, instead of 8%). The study also identified a “definite causal relationship” with degeneration of the disc above the LSTV.

  277. Porter NA, Lalam RK, Tins BJ, et al. Prevalence of extraforaminal nerve root compression below lumbosacral transitional vertebrae. Skeletal Radiology. 2014 Jan;43(1):55–60. PubMed 24310344 ❐
  278. Apazidis A, Ricart PA, Diefenbach CM, Spivak JM. The prevalence of transitional vertebrae in the lumbar spine. Spine J. 2011 Sep;11(9):858–62. PubMed 21951610 ❐

    This study looked at how common transitional vertebrae are, and whether or not they correlate with low back pain. The researchers concluded that this deformity is not much more common in people with back pain, and possibly no more common at all: “Although LSTV’s role in low back pain remains controversial, our study has shown that, when the same criteria are used for classification, prevalence among the general population and symptomatic patients may be similar.”

    (See more detailed commentary on this paper.)

  279. For some reason, even though it makes perfect linguistic sense, peripheral nerves are not referred to as the “branches” of nerve roots.
  280. The spinal cord proper terminates surprisingly high up, around L1, in a fairly well-defined bulb, the conus medullaris, from which many fine fibers emerge — very much like a horsetail. Inside the spinal column, the fibres are know as the cauda equina. Groups of these fibres branch off and pass through holes in the spine, emerging as nerve roots.
  281. It’s “just” lumbar myelopathy if it’s chronic and minor, but more worrisome CES if is abrupt and severe. But it depends on who you talk to. These concepts and labels are an overlapping mess. Hoeritzauer et al in 2020: “The variation in definitions and reporting of diagnostic criteria likely reflects the lack of agreed definitions and multiple classifications of CES in use clinically and in the literature.”
  282. Angus M, Curtis-Lopez CM, Carrasco R, et al. Determination of potential risk characteristics for cauda equina compression in emergency department patients presenting with atraumatic back pain: a 4-year retrospective cohort analysis within a tertiary referral neurosciences centre. Emerg Med J. 2021 Oct. PubMed 34642235 ❐
  283. A digital rectal exam, assessing anal “grip strength,” has been a standard test since forever. Happily, it turns out that this is probably not a reliable indicator of CES. Angus et al. on anuses: “We found no relationship between digital rectal examination findings and the diagnosis of CES.”

    Not that there’s anything wrong with a digital rectal exam when it’s called for, but I think we can all agree it’s nice to skip it when we can.

  284. Fairbank J, Hashimoto R, Dailey A, Patel AA, Dettori JR. Does patient history and physical examination predict MRI proven cauda equina syndrome? Evid Based Spine Care J. 2011 Nov;2(4):27–33. PubMed 23230403 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52216 ❐
  285. The answer is probably just that a wide variety of other things that can cause similar symptoms. But a more interesting and speculative possibility is that the cauda equina might be erratically vulnerable (for biological reasons) to quite subtle compression, resulting in an ephemeral form of CES caused by compression, technically, but too subtle to readily confirm with a scan. It is technically CES, it’s just hard to confirm and less scary and fades away as the vulnerability naturally resolves.

    I decided it was worth exploring this in a footnote because it’s so closely related to why radiculopathy — much more common — can also be so weird.

  286. Tomjesson.substack.com [Internet]. Jesson T. How rare is cauda equina syndrome?; 2021 November 17 [cited 23 Apr 13]. PainSci Bibliography 52215 ❐

    Tom Jesson went looking for the origins of the widespread claim that cauda equina syndrome is extremely rare — so rare that a family doctor will see only “one case in their career.” He found very little! It is, “with many degrees of separation, based on a Slovenian paper that probably under-estimates the incidence of CES.” There is not much hard data, but what he found (mostly summed up by Hoeritzauer 2020 suggests that CES is indeed rare, but probably nowhere near that rare: clinicians that help people with musculoskeletal health regularly “can expect to see about a dozen cases of CES.”

    It’s important to think through what “rare” means, because rare can either mean “so rare you can practically forget about it” or “rare but there; and you will see it—more than once!”. CES is the latter.

  287. Hoeritzauer I, Wood M, Copley PC, Demetriades AK, Woodfield J. What is the incidence of cauda equina syndrome? A systematic review. J Neurosurg Spine. 2020 Feb:1–10. PubMed 32059184 ❐
  288. Konstantinou K, Lewis M, Dunn KM. Agreement of self-reported items and clinically assessed nerve root involvement (or sciatica) in a primary care setting. Eur Spine J. 2012 Nov;21(11):2306–15. PubMed 22752591 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52894 ❐
  289. Slipman CW, Plastaras CT, Palmitier RA, Huston CW, Sterenfeld EB. Symptom provocation of fluoroscopically guided cervical nerve root stimulation. Are dynatomal maps identical to dermatomal maps? Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1998 Oct;23(20):2235–42. PubMed 9802168 ❐
  290. Taylor CS, Coxon AJ, Watson PC, Greenough CG. Do L5 and s1 nerve root compressions produce radicular pain in a dermatomal pattern? Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2013 May;38(12):995–8. PubMed 23324941 ❐
  291. Murphy DR, Hurwitz EL, Gerrard JK, Clary R. Pain patterns and descriptions in patients with radicular pain: does the pain necessarily follow a specific dermatome? Chiropr Osteopat. 2009;17:9. PubMed 19772560 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53423 ❐
  292. Rainville J, Laxer E, Keel J, et al. Exploration of sensory impairments associated with C6 and C7 radiculopathies. Spine J. 2016 Jan;16(1):49–54. PubMed 26253986 ❐

    The dermatome patterns most professionals are familiar with were established many decades ago, and were not studied much again until the 21st Century. This study carefully checked the exact location of symptoms in 120 patients with suspected radiculopathy (symptoms in a dermatomal pattern, caused by nerve root compression). Perhaps unsurprisingly, they found that the dermatomal patterns were not as precise as the old maps would lead us to believe, and exhibit significant overlap, “to the extent that caution should be exercised when predicting compression of either the C6 or C7 nerve roots based on locations of impaired sensation.”

    (See more detailed commentary on this paper.)

  293. Goldstein B. Anatomic issues related to cervical and lumbosacral radiculopathy. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. 2002 Aug;13(3):423–37. PubMed 12380543 ❐
  294. Tanaka N, Fujimoto Y, An HS, Ikuta Y, Yasuda M. The anatomic relation among the nerve roots, intervertebral foramina, and intervertebral discs of the cervical spine. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2000 Feb;25(3):286–91. PubMed 10703098 ❐
  295. Loynes RD. Scoliosis after thoracoplasty. J Bone Joint Surg Br. 1972 Aug;54(3):484–98. PubMed 5053892 ❐ “Thoracoplasty” is an obsolete surgical technique, just barely the lesser of evils, intended to collapse the chest and close lung cavities carved out by severe tuberculosis. This was mostly achieved by removing ribs, but there were other tactics.
  296. McAviney J, Roberts C, Sullivan B, et al. The prevalence of adult de novo scoliosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Spine J. 2020 12;29(12):2960–2969. PubMed 32440771 ❐
  297. Shakil H, Iqbal ZA, Al-Ghadir AH. Scoliosis: review of types of curves, etiological theories and conservative treatment. J Back Musculoskelet Rehabil. 2014;27(2):111–5. PubMed 24284269 ❐

    This is one of the only modern papers I know of that is ostensibly about the causes of scoliosis — if you can call about three paragraphs of uninspired speculation a “focus.”

  298. Théroux J, Le May S, Hebert JJ, Labelle H. Back Pain Prevalence Is Associated With Curve-type and Severity in Adolescents With Idiopathic Scoliosis: A Cross-sectional Study. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2017 Aug;42(15):E914–E919. PubMed 27870807 ❐
  299. Wong AYL, Samartzis D, Cheung PWH, Cheung JPY. How Common Is Back Pain and What Biopsychosocial Factors Are Associated With Back Pain in Patients With Adolescent Idiopathic Scoliosis? Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2019 04;477(4):676–686. PubMed 30516661 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51404 ❐
  300. Nachemson A. Adult scoliosis and back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1979;4(6):513–7. PubMed 160083 ❐
  301. Yuan W, Shen J, Chen L, et al. Differences in Nonspecific Low Back Pain between Young Adult Females with and without Lumbar Scoliosis. Pain Res Manag. 2019;2019:9758273. PubMed 30944687 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51443 ❐
  302. Gremeaux V, Casillas JM, Fabbro-Peray P, et al. Analysis of low back pain in adults with scoliosis. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2008 Feb;33(4):402–5. PubMed 18277872 ❐
  303. The threat wasn’t immediate, but more of an emergency in slow-motion. She was actually asymptomatic at the time of the surgery. However, her organs were already compressed, and the situation would only get worse and more difficult to repair.
  304. In my experience, patients with pronounced scoliosis have less anxiety about whatever pain they have, probably because it has an obvious cause. They understand it, they are more resigned to it, and they are less worried about the future of it. This lack of anxiety might actually account for a great amount of the difference in suffering between a patient like Becky and people with more acute and “scary” low back pain.
  305. Karachalios T, Sofianos J, Roidis N, et al. Ten-year follow-up evaluation of a school screening program for scoliosis. Is the forward-bending test an accurate diagnostic criterion for the screening of scoliosis? Spine. 1999 Nov;24(22):2318–2324.
  306. Known as the Valsalva maneuver, it’s just a blocked but forced exhalation.  Sticking your thumb in your mouth like a cork and then blowing strongly without releasing air will increase your abdominal pressure.
  307. By far the most common location is the sacroiliac region near the superior portion of the iliac crest. It’s so common that they are sometimes called “episacroiliac lipomas.” Other common locations: upper back, shoulders, and abdomen. But they are extremely common in the low back, so much so that you’d be hard pressed to find anyone over 40 who doesn’t have one or two.
  308. Probably of the cluneal nerves, especially the superior cluneal nerve, which emerges from the deep tissues of the lumbar spine, passes through tiny holes in the thoracolumbar fascia, and then over the ridge of the sacroiliac crest. They innervate the skin of the buttocks, so most likely mild cluneal nerve impingement would cause a minor superficial pain in the ass. But it might not be limited to that.
  309. Erdem HR, Nacır B, Özeri Z, Karagöz A. [Episacral lipoma: a treatable cause of low back pain]. Agri. 2013;25(2):83–6. PubMed 23720083 ❐
  310. Bicket MC, Simmons C, Zheng Y. The Best-Laid Plans of "Back Mice" and Men: A Case Report and Literature Review of Episacroiliac Lipoma. Pain Physician. 2016 Mar;19(3):181–8. PubMed 27008292 ❐
  311. Not the standard lipoma,” he explains, but “a soft benign non-painful fatty tumor in the midst of the fat below the skin. Rather, it is a firm nodule of fat protruding up through a small opening in the deep fascia covering the back muscles. Often, the nodules are multiple, resembling a cluster of grapes.”
  312. Erdem et al claim that lipomas are “tender.” This is a blatantly incorrect generalization about lipomas — I’ve palpated hundreds of them, and almost no tender ones at all, and even the few tender ones are not very tender. They also declare that lipomas are a “significant” cause of back pain, which is clearly going too far. So there are real reasons to question their judgement.
  313. Trentu.ca [Internet]. Coughlan R. Technology Idolatory: An Exploration of Healthcare's Love Affair with Machines That Go "Bing"; 2004 Jan [cited 15 Mar 17]. PainSci Bibliography 56019 ❐

    “There is something wonderfully compelling about new technology whether it comes in the shape of new cars, kitchen appliances or technological advancements in the modern clinic. I want to briefly explore how our values and beliefs concerning technology may contribute to some problematic aspects of modern medical practice.”

  314. Webster BS, Bauer AZ, Choi Y, Cifuentes M, Pransky GS. Iatrogenic consequences of early magnetic resonance imaging in acute, work-related, disabling low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2013 Oct;38(22):1939–46. PubMed 23883826 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53341 ❐ “Early MRI without indication has a strong iatrogenic effect in acute LBP, regardless of radiculopathy status. Providers and patients should be made aware that when early MRI is not indicated, it provides no benefits, and worse outcomes are likely.”
  315. Laswi M, Lesperance R, Kaye A, et al. Redefining the costal margin: A pilot study. J Trauma Acute Care Surg. 2022 Dec;93(6):762–766. PubMed 36121266 ❐
  316. There are two main ways that Laswi et al. may have found more hypermobility in the cadavers they studied than actually exists in living people, both of which they acknowledged. First, their subjects were quite old, an average age of 83 years at the time of death. Second, their dissection techniques could have actually created some of the hypermobility they observed. But none of that would affect their most amazing result: the extremely high incidence of floating tenths.
  317. Strange, wonderful, and problematic anatomical variations occur in humans all the time. The best anatomical diagrams depict average anatomy only, and sometimes they are even wrong about what’s average — which is what this post is all about. The line between “normal variation” and “defect” is very blurry. We often make a fuss over the visible anatomical variations — too much of a fuss in many cases (“structuralism”) — while neglecting more common invisible oddities that may well matter much more. Such cases often join the “X-files” of therapy, the unsolved clinical mysteries that every pro encounters. Floating ribs are an excellent candidate example of this. “For every visible, superficial oddity, there may be an invisible internal one… and only surgeons ever find out about them” (Dr. Sherwin Nuland). See You Might Just Be Weird.
  318. For instance, a modern edition of the classic textbook, Gray’s Anatomy, shows and definitively describes only two floaters in numerous diagrams, with no mention of variation at all. Same with Netter’s Atlas of Human Anatomy, which is next in line for “most famous anatomy text.” I have both of these texts in my library, and I dusted them off for this (literally).
  319. Shimaguchi S. Tenth rib is floating in Japanese. Anatomischer Anzeiger. 1974;135(1-2):72–82. PubMed 4416068 ❐
  320. Mysteriously, PubMed answered my search for floating rib data with just one other weird item, in addition to the Japanese study: “Insect succession on a decomposing piglet carcass placed in a man-made freshwater pond in Malaysia,” which chronicles the lives of insects on said floating carcass for, you guessed it, ten days. “The carcass along with the maggots sunk on day tenth [sic, and probably the reason for the weird result], leaving an oily layer on the water surface.” Search tech is tricky.
  321. Fares MY, Dimassi Z, Baydoun H, Musharrafieh U. Slipping Rib Syndrome: Solving the Mystery of the Shooting Pain. Am J Med Sci. 2019 Feb;357(2):168–173. PubMed 30509726 ❐
  322. Both Tietze and costochondritis affect the junction of the sternum with costal cartilage. Tietze tends to be worse: it’s associated with ominous causes like infection, autoimmune disease, cancer, and occurs mostly in the upper ribcage. Costochondritis is more like an arthritic flare-up: milder and more evenly distributed. Slipped rib syndrome is clearly a sibling condition, but it doesn't affect a connection between rib and sternum.
  323. The costovertebral joints in back may also get irritated, with or without subluxation — in the same way that I don’t have to dislocate a knuckle for it to ache with arthritis. But Laswi et al. only studied the costal margin.
  324. That was the number whether the rib was floating or not, and not all of those subluxations were internal subluxations. Not all subluxations displace the rib inward (which is the only way an intercostal nerve can get squeezed.)
  325. A more exact description by Laswi et al.: “In the experience of the authors caring for patients with slipped rib syndrome, many of these patients have a ‘hook’ tip of the 10th rib. A ‘hook’ tip on the 10th rib is a cartilaginous tapering to the rib that curves up towards the 9th rib at an acute angle compared with the normal curvature of the 10th rib.”
  326. A proper ontological audit would call for a whole ‘nother article, and I will probably write it someday. But today is not that day. For now, I will just say that I have seen enough evidence-based reasons to suspect that rib subluxations are a legit phenomenon … along with an awful lot of anecdotes that are hard to explain without rib subluxations. Including quite a few of my own experiences. I have much less faith that anyone can reliably put those joints back where they belong, but I’m now quite confident that the need is real.
  327. Machado LAC, Kamper SJ, Herbert RD, Maher CG, McAuley JH. Analgesic effects of treatments for non-specific low back pain: a meta-analysis of placebo-controlled randomized trials. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2009 May;48(5):520–7. PubMed 19109315 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 54670 ❐ There is a great deal of back pain science to review, but the authors of this review found that shockingly little of it was worth their while: just 34 acceptable studies out of a 1031 candidates, and even among those “trial quality was highly variable.” Their conclusions are derived from only the best sort of scientific experiments: not just the gold-standard of randomized and placebo-controlled tests, but carefully choosing only the “right” kind of placebos (several kinds of placebos were grounds for disqualification, because of their known potential to skew the results). They do a good job of explaining exactly how and why they picked the studies they did.
  328. It’s not that the science hasn’t been done yet, though in a few cases the “more study needed” cliché does apply. Most of these treatments have now been studied enough to know.
  329. Artus M, van der Windt DA, Jordan KP, Hay EM. Low back pain symptoms show a similar pattern of improvement following a wide range of primary care treatments: a systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2010 Dec;49(12):2346–56. PubMed 20713495 ❐

    “Symptoms seem to improve in a similar pattern in clinical trials following a wide variety of active as well as inactive treatments.” That is, back pain patients improve with or without treatment. See Back pain for detailed analysis by Dr. Neil O’Connell. Note that a follow-up study in 2014 established that participating in an RCT isn’t the “active ingredient” in the observed improvements — on average, everyone improves about the same speed/amount, regardless of whether they are being studied or not (see Artus).

  330. Artus M, van der Windt D, Jordan KP, Croft PR. The clinical course of low back pain: a meta-analysis comparing outcomes in randomised clinical trials (RCTs) and observational studies. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2014 Mar;15:68. PubMed 24607083 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53064 ❐

    Strictly speaking, this study does not show that back pain gets better with or without treatment — but that is a nearly inescapable implication. What it was really about is a fairly technical point about research: does participation in a randomized controlled trial produce different results? Do people who participate in studies do better than people who do not? Answer: nope. This exhaustive meta-analysis determined that basically everyone follows exactly the same pattern of improvement in back pain regardless of whether they are involved in an RCT.

    But for this to be true, it must also be true that most treatments are mostly not affecting the progression of back pain. If some treatments worked, then some RCTs would be producing evidence of faster and better recovery. The authors here warn against trying to interpret such diverse data in this way, but it strikes me as an token caution — the findings here tend to reinforce past findings by the same authors (see Artus), namely that no treatment clearly works for back pain.

  331. What if a treatment only works well for just the right sort of patients? This is the popular “sub-grouping” criticism of back pain science so far (and a common form of advanced special pleading by advocates for unproven therapies). Unfortunately Machado et al did not find a pattern of superior results from careful subgrouping, and concluded that the evidence is contradictory at best — even after the most productive decade of back pain science in history. It’s also been shown that “the credibility of subgroup claims in back pain trials is usually low” (see Saragiotto 2016, Saragiotto 2017).
  332. Another objection: “Some authors have argued that the small effects of treatments for acute non-specific low back pain are a consequence of the favourable natural history … . The theory is that, at the conclusion of treatment in trials, control groups have improved substantially and so there is not ‘room’ for large treatment effects.” I think there are some rather glaring logical problems with this idea, but hard data is more convincing: Machado et al found that this “does not seem consistent with the data” and not only is there “room” for treatment effects, “There seems to be considerable scope for large treatment effects but how this can be achieved at present is unclear.” No kidding.
  333. Henschke N, Ostelo RW, van Tulder MW, et al. Behavioural treatment for chronic low-back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010;(7):CD002014. PubMed 20614428 ❐

    This review of the results of dozens of scientific studies shows that behavioural therapies for low back pain have generally been failing the “impress me” test. It is possible that behavioural therapy is more effective for a certain kind of patient. However, if so, apparently there are not enough of those kinds of patients, or the effect is not big enough, to have any discernible effect on the average results of experiments. If evidence of a benefit is being “washed out,” it is being washed out rather easily. Behavioural therapy might work, a little, for some, but scraps of efficacy hardly seem worth fighting over.

  334. Vibe-Fersum K, O’Sullivan P, Skouen JS, Smith A, Kvåle A. Efficacy of classification-based cognitive functional therapy in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain: A randomized controlled trial. Eur J Pain. 2013 Jul;17(6):916–28. PubMed 23208945 ❐ Classification-based cognitive functional therapy (CFT) was tested on 62 patients with moderate back pain, and compared to 59 who were treated with manual therapy and exercise. CFT consisting of “reframing the persons’ understanding of their back pain in a person-centred manner, with an emphasis on changing maladaptive movement, cognitive and lifestyle behaviours contributing to their vicious cycle of pain.” Three months and a year later, the CFT group was much better off: a 13-point boost on a 100-point disability scale, and 3 points on a 10-point pain scale. Those are not amazing results, but they’re clinically significant, and they beat manual therapy and exercise handily (those patients improved by only 5.5 and 1.5 points on the same scales). CFT was “more effective at reducing pain, disability, fear beliefs, mood and sick leave at long-term follow-up than MT-EX.”
  335. It’s rare to get both in one study: statistical confidence about an effect that’s big enough to be meaningful to patients. In fact, that’s the take-home message from Machado: as of 2009, it had never happened in any study of back pain. (Clarification) Some of the treatment effect sizes in other studies were large enough to be considered clinically significant, but only just.
  336. Deep thought alert — it could be that manual therapy and exercise only did as well as they did in this test insofar as they also reduce fear and anxiety. Exercise in particular can build confidence, depending on how it is prescribed and performed.
  337. “Proven” would be an exaggeration, but these results can be safely considered “promising” while we wait for replication from bigger studies. And it’s important to note that CFT is a mostly harmless solution, safe and cheap — you can’t go too far wrong with this approach to back pain.
  338. Sarno J. Mind Over Back Pain: A radically new approach to the diagnosis and treatment of back pain. Trade paperback, red/blue cover ed. Berkley Books; 1999. p94.
  339. Brison RJ, Hartling L, Dostaler S. A randomized controlled trial of an educational intervention to prevent the chronic pain of whiplash associated disorders following rear-end motor vehicle collisions. Spine. 2005 Aug 15;30(16):1799–807. PubMed 16103847 ❐

    This is one of a few studies showing a benefit to education for neck pain specifically. Researchers showed a reassuring educational video to more than 200 patients with “whiplash associated disorders” (i.e. whiplash injuries that become chronic neck cricks), and found that they had less severe symptoms than patients who received no educational intervention. The effectiveness of education probably depends a lot on the type of neck pain and the type of education, making it very hard to study. A recent review of the scientific literature found that most such studies are negative (see Haines or Ainpradub), but I believe that there are still reasons to be optimistic about education for pain problems. Above all, it depends on the type and quality of the education! The right education may be effective, and the wrong could even be harmful. The fact that some education has been shown to be beneficial is promising.

  340. Schultz IZ, Crook J, Meloche GR, Berkowitz J, et al. Psychosocial factors predictive of occupational low back disability: towards development of a return-to-work model. Pain. 2004 Jan:77–85. PubMed 14715392 ❐

    This study identified factors affecting return-to-work time after an episode of low-back pain. From the abstract: “The key psychosocial predictors identified were expectations of recovery and perception of health change.”

  341. Moseley 2018, op. cit.
  342. That certainly sounds like “all in your head!” But it’s still not — not in the sense that it’s a psychological problem, imaginary, or psychosomatic. What Lorimer means is that pain is a neurological construct: all pain is “built” by the brain based on an interpretation of incoming signals. We know this very clearly about vision. We know that the raw signals from our eyes are virtually useless until they are turned upside down and backwards, rearranged and color-corrected, and thoroughly processed by the brain until finally we get to “see” something. It’s no different with pain. Every bit of pain that seems to be in our body is actually just an ugly picture painted by our brains — an illusion.
  343. Nijs J, Paul van Wilgen C, Van Oosterwijck J, van Ittersum M, Meeus M. How to explain central sensitization to patients with 'unexplained' chronic musculoskeletal pain: practice guidelines. Man Ther. 2011 Oct;16(5):413–8. PubMed 21632273 ❐

    This is an expert opinion piece echoing some of the tenets of the “explain pain” movement (see also: Moseley).

  344. Ingraham. Placebo Power Hype: The placebo effect is fascinating, but its “power” isn’t all it’s cracked up to be.  ❐ PainScience.com. 8114 words.
  345. A great example from my mail bag: a young reader with harsh back pain asked me if the answer to his pain is to study yoga. Full time. For a year. In India! I’d say no. Probably not. Unless he’s Indian. Unless studying back pain full-time in India was a genuine dream for him before the back pain, for other reasons. It is not clear that yoga deserves an investment of four hours a week … let alone a complete lifestyle overhaul.
  346. Bower JE, Irwin MR. Mind-body therapies and control of inflammatory biology: A descriptive review. Brain Behav Immun. 2016 Jan;51:1–11. PubMed 26116436 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53640 ❐

    This is a qualitative review of 26 randomized controlled trials of the biological effects of mind-body therapies like Tai Chi, Qigong, yoga, and meditation. The studies show “mixed effects” on inflammation (CRP, IL-6, stimulated cytokine production, etc), and more consistent results for “genomic markers.” Based on this evidence, it seems likely that these activities are meaningfully good for you, and probably helpful for some kinds of chronic pain.

  347. Farias M, Wikholm C. Has the science of mindfulness lost its mind? BJPsych Bull. 2016 Dec;40(6):329–332. PubMed 28377813 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 53075 ❐
  348. People keep writing to me, “Tch tch, Paul, yoga is totally mainstream!” But I disagree. If you live in downtown Vancouver, it’s certainly mainstream. Yes, you can buy yoga mats at Walmart. But I grew up in a small blue collar town, and I can assure you, yoga still seems veeeeery weird to a lot of folks who aren’t living in the throbbing urban centers of our civilization. And there’s plenty of folks who moved to the big city not that long ago. Big Macs are mainstream. Yoga is not.
  349. Although alternative medicine is certainly now a substantial industry, it is still dwarfed by the size of mainstream medicine, and its popularity is exaggerated by the people selling the alternatives. Acupuncture, for instance, is nowhere near as widely utilized in North America as North American acupuncturists would have us believe.
  350. The kernel of this joke is not mine, but I can’t figure out where it came from originally: there are multiple variations from multiple sources.
  351. Cramer H, Lauche R, Haller H, Dobos G. A systematic review and meta-analysis of yoga for low back pain. Clin J Pain. 2013 May;29(5):450–60. PubMed 23246998 ❐

    A great citation for yoga studios to put on their bulletin boards: “strong evidence for short-term effectiveness and moderate evidence for long-term effectiveness of yoga for chronic low back pain.”

  352. Cochrane is actually a bit notorious among skeptics for never actually arriving at a richly deserved negative conclusion, because more study is always allegedly needed. This is exploited by cranks and quacks who spin it as a meaningful absence of evidence. “So … you’re saying there’s a chance!” And the Cochrane reviews seem to affirm that: “Technically, we cannot crush your hopes at this time.” And so evidence-based medicine weirdly manages to give a kind of a pass to all kinds of dubious remedies and therapies. That is not really how EBM was supposed to work. For more on this theme, see Why “Science”-Based Instead of “Evidence”-Based?.
  353. Wieland LS, Skoetz N, Pilkington K, et al. Yoga treatment for chronic non-specific low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017 01;1:CD010671. PubMed 28076926 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51437 ❐ Yes, “yoga compared to non-exercise controls results in small to moderate improvements in back-related function.” But “…the effect size did not meet predefined levels of minimum clinical importance.”
  354. Anheyer D, Haller H, Lauche R, Dobos G, Cramer H. Yoga for treating low back pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain. 2022 04;163(4):e504–e517. PubMed 34326296 ❐ It’s a common bit of jiggery pokery in science to fail to point out small “effect sizes,” and this is a perfect example.
  355. Zhu F, Zhang M, Wang D, et al. Yoga compared to non-exercise or physical therapy exercise on pain, disability, and quality of life for patients with chronic low back pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS One. 2020;15(9):e0238544. PubMed 32870936 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51433 ❐ Zhu et al introduce their paper with the bold premise that “Yoga has been proven to be an effective therapy for chronic low back pain.” Really? Because that same paper concludes that the benefit of yoga compared to not exercising is so minor that the best you could say was that it “might” be superior. “Might be better” and “proven” do not belong in the same abstract! So the “proven” benefit is getting into DWFP territory — “damned with faint praise,” so common with back pain treatment that I wish I could get away with abbreviating it with no explanation.
  356. Michalsen A, Jeitler M, Kessler CS, et al. Yoga, Eurythmy Therapy and Standard Physiotherapy (YES-Trial) for Patients With Chronic Non-specific Low Back Pain: A Three-Armed Randomized Controlled Trial. J Pain. 2021 10;22(10):1233–1245. PubMed 33892154 ❐
  357. Neyaz O, Sumila L, Nanda S, Wadhwa S. Effectiveness of Hatha Yoga Versus Conventional Therapeutic Exercises for Chronic Nonspecific Low-Back Pain. J Altern Complement Med. 2019 Sep;25(9):938–945. PubMed 31347920 ❐
  358. Joyce C, Roseen EJ, Keysor JJ, et al. Can Yoga or Physical Therapy for Chronic Low Back Pain Improve Depression and Anxiety Among Adults From a Racially Diverse, Low-Income Community? A Secondary Analysis of a Randomized Controlled Trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2021 06;102(6):1049–1058. PubMed 33556352 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51432 ❐
  359. Sherman KJ, Cherkin DC, Wellman RD, et al. A Randomized Trial Comparing Yoga, Stretching, and a Self-care Book for Chronic Low Back Pain. Arch Intern Med. 2011 Oct. PubMed 22025101 ❐
  360. Brämberg EB, Bergström G, Jensen I, Hagberg J, Kwak L. Effects of yoga, strength training and advice on back pain: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2017 03;18(1):132. PubMed 28356091 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51436 ❐
  361. Roseen EJ, Gerlovin H, Felson DT, et al. Which Chronic Low Back Pain Patients Respond Favorably to Yoga, Physical Therapy, and a Self-care Book? Responder Analyses from a Randomized Controlled Trial. Pain Med. 2021 02;22(1):165–180. PubMed 32662833 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51438 ❐ About 40% of people responded to yoga or physical therapy, and “predictors of response included having more than a high school education, a higher income, employment, few depressive symptoms, lower perceived stress, few work-related fear avoidance beliefs, high pain self-efficacy, and being a nonsmoker.”
  362. Saragiotto BT, Maher CG, Hancock MJ, Koes BW. Subgrouping Patients With Nonspecific Low Back Pain: Hope or Hype? J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2017 Feb;47(2):44–48. PubMed 28142361 ❐
  363. Saragiotto BT, Yamato TP, Maher C. Yoga for low back pain: PEDro systematic review update. Br J Sports Med. 2015 Oct;49(20):1351. PubMed 25082615 ❐
  364. Rabey M, Smith A, Kent P, et al. Chronic low back pain is highly individualised: patterns of classification across three unidimensional subgrouping analyses. Scand J Pain. 2019 Jun. PubMed 31256070 ❐ This is actually the gist of a scientific paper: subgrouping of low back pain doesn’t work out in because “it’s complicated.”
  365. Wieland 2017, op. cit.
  366. Tilbrook HE, Cox H, Hewitt CE, et al. Yoga for chronic low back pain: a randomized trial. Ann Intern Med. 2011 Nov;155(9):569–78. PubMed 22041945 ❐
  367. NYTimes.com [Internet]. Broad W. How Yoga Can Wreck Your Body; 2012 January [cited 14 Jan 6]. PainSci Bibliography 55108 ❐
  368. There are yoga apologists? Oh, yes! Seriously. Yoga has many characteristics of a religion, after all, and has always inspired some passionate devotees with very strongly held beliefs, and you cannot criticize yoga without provoking them.
  369. After a too-positive initial review on Facebook, many of my readers pointed out valid science-based criticisms, primarily that Broad relies heavily on anecdotes. In particular, he concludes the piece with a doozy based on pure speculation: that decades of yoga was the direct cause of a severe case of spinal stenosis, which is not a safe assumption at all (stenosis happens, with or without yoga). The worst-case scenario is that the article is fear mongering based mostly on a handful of nasty anecdotes without citing much in the way of real risk/benefit data. For instance, for all we know, average yoga injuries per hour may be less than soccer, or even showering ... and we can’t do a real risk-benefit analysis without that information.
  370. Telles S, Bhardwaj AK, Gupta RK, et al. A Randomized Controlled Trial to Assess Pain and Magnetic Resonance Imaging-Based (MRI-Based) Structural Spine Changes in Low Back Pain Patients After Yoga Practice. Med Sci Monit. 2016 Sep;22:3228–47. PubMed 27619104 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 51346 ❐ This unusual 2016 study actually measured changes in the spine itself — and finding none at all. Incredibly, the experiment required an hour of yoga per day for three months — a lot of yoga! It does seem like it might take quite a large dosage to have a measurable effect on the spine. Unfortunately (but unsurprisingly), the regimen “did not alter MRI-proven changes in the intervertebral discs and in the vertebrae.”
  371. Viana da Silva P, Kamper SJ, Robson E, et al. 'Myths and facts' education is comparable to 'facts only' for recall of back pain information but may improve fear-avoidance beliefs: an embedded randomized trial. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 2022 Jul:1–29. PubMed 35802818 ❐
  372. The pain becomes more focal, retreating from the periphery, from distal to proximal as well as lateral to medial. Mr. Poulter explains: “‘Central’ is often mistaken for ‘middle’ and therapists think they have to produce symptoms in the middle of the spine in order to have centralisation… even if the patient’s symptoms originated in their buttock.”
  373. May 2018, op. cit.
  374. Long A, Donelson R, Fung T. Does it matter which exercise? A randomized control trial of exercise for low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2004 Dec;29(23):2593–602. PubMed 15564907 ❐
  375. The Health Effects of Cannabis and Cannabinoids: The Current State of Evidence and Recommendations for Research 2017, op. cit.
  376. Nugent SM, Morasco BJ, O’Neil ME, et al. The Effects of Cannabis Among Adults With Chronic Pain and an Overview of General Harms: A Systematic Review. Ann Intern Med. 2017 Aug. PubMed 28806817 ❐

    This review of 27 scientific trials of cannabis for chronic pain trials is disappointing: weakly positive for neuropathic pain, and just inconclusive otherwise, with some evidence of risks of short term mental fog, car accidents, and psychosis. This conclusion is at odds with other recent reviews and reports, which have offered more optimistic interpretations of mostly the same evidence (most notably The Health Effects of Cannabis and Cannabinoids).

    In addition to finding very little benefit for pain, the review also reports some evidence of risks of short term mental fog, car accidents, and psychosis.
  377. Häuser W, Petzke F. [Evidence of the efficacy and safety of cannabis medicines for chronic pain management: A methodological minefield]. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2019 Jul;62(7):836–844. PubMed 31139839 ❐
  378. Bebee B, Taylor DM, Bourke E, et al. The CANBACK trial: a randomised, controlled clinical trial of oral cannabidiol for people presenting to the emergency department with acute low back pain. Med J Aust. 2021 05;214(8):370–375. PubMed 33846971 ❐
  379. Non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (ibuprofen, are actually quite dangerous — they are hard on the body at any dose. Acetaminophen/Paracetamol, on the other hand, is one of the safest drugs there has ever been at recommended dosages (it abuses the liver in large quantities). CBD is likely safer than the former, and likely not as safe as the latter. To be a viable alternative to NSAIDs, it just has to have at least the same effect. But it can’t actually beat acetaminophen on safety, so it would have to be much more effective — and while possible, it seems very unlikely based on this test.
  380. Young, K and Sofair, A. “CDC Reports Breakthrough in Vaping-Linked Lung Injury Investigation.” Nov 9, 2019. Accessed on 2019-11-23.

    Vitamin E acetate has been detected for the first time in the lungs of patients who vaped and developed serious lung injury. Vitamin E acetate is a thick, oily substance added to some e-liquids, particularly those containing tetrahydrocannabinol (THC).

    “These new findings are significant because for the first time we have detected a potential toxin of concern ... from biological samples,” CDC Principal Deputy Director Dr. Anne Schuchat said on Friday. Previously, vitamin E acetate had been detected in product samples.

  381. Ware MA, Wang T, Shapiro S, Collet JP; COMPASS study team. Cannabis for the Management of Pain: Assessment of Safety Study (COMPASS). J Pain. 2015 Dec;16(12):1233–42. PubMed 26385201 ❐

    Although we already have many reasons to suspect that cannabis usage is very safe, the more data the better on this topic, and particularly in the context of treating non-cancer pain. The results are good news, and completely consistent with other evidence. Ars Technica:

    Almost every news story one reads about the use of cannabis as a medical therapy contains some variation of disclaimer saying ‘more research is needed’ into the longterm safety of medical cannabis use. Now a tiny bit of that ‘more research’ has been published in the Journal of Pain. The headline result was that there was NO INCREASE IN THE NUMBER OF SERIOUS ADVERSE EVENTS in a group that used cannabis for chronic pain when compared to a group that did not.

    It’s hard to overstate how significant that kind of safety level is for any medication that helps with pain. Even the mildest over-the-counter analgesics come with serious risks (see How risky are NSAIDS?). Cannabis is not risk free — this study did find evidence of non-serious adverse events — but the total absence of serious adverse events is a big deal.

    (By the way, this science comes from Canada, which is where I come from. You’re welcome.)

  382. Be your own scientist. Take each major type of drug on three separate but relatively similar situations to try to minimize confounding factors. Rate your pain on a scale of 10 before you take the pill, an hour later, and three hours later — and maybe add a note the next day about how the whole day went.
  383. Machado GC, Maher CG, Ferreira PH, et al. Non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs for spinal pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann Rheum Dis. 2017 Jul;76(7):1269–1278. PubMed 28153830 ❐
  384. Derry S, Wiffen PJ, Moore RA, Bendtsen L. Ibuprofen for acute treatment of episodic tension-type headache in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;7:CD011474. PubMed 26230487 ❐ Ibuprofen seems to work quite well for a few lucky winners: a substantial beneficial effect in about 1 in 6 people, especially for harsher headaches, the ones that start faster and stronger. About one in 14 people will get “complete” relief — not bad odds for the desperate. The average effect is unimpressive, but for a few “responders” it’s good medicine. The same could be true of back pain… or not.
  385. Medication-overuse headaches (MOH), formerly known as “rebound” headaches, are probably mostly caused by dependence-and-withdrawal physiology, like getting a headache when you quit drinking coffee, but it might be more complicated. Pain-killers taken for headaches may be a surprisingly common and ironic cause of headaches (though maybe less of a plague than some headlines have led us to fear; see Scher). This topic is obviously of special interest to patients with unexplained headaches, and so I discuss it a lot in my headache guide, but it’s also just a major side effect for anyone treating anything with pain-killers long-term.
  386. Ashbrook J, Rogdakis N, Callaghan MJ, Yeowell G, Goodwin PC. The therapeutic management of back pain with and without sciatica in the emergency department: a systematic review. Physiotherapy. 2020 Jul;109:13–32. PubMed 32846282 ❐
  387. Traeger AC, Buchbinder R, Harris IA, Clavisi OM, Maher CG. Avoid routinely prescribing medicines for non-specific low back pain. Br J Sports Med. 2018 Mar. PubMed 29514824 ❐
  388. Oral diclofenac is nasty stuff: it is associated with horrible cardiovascular risks and should probably be banned (McGettigan et al). But the dosage from topical use is much smaller and safer (How risky are NSAIDS?).
  389. Pre-emptive rebuttal … before someone tells me, yes, I know we shouldn’t make too much of a study done on rats. But the stakes are low, and that’s the key: an anti-inflammatory gel was not an unreasonable or unsafe thing to try before this evidence, and this just gives it a boost. It makes it more reasonable.
  390. Huang ZJ, Hsu E, Li HC, et al. Topical application of compound Ibuprofen suppresses pain by inhibiting sensory neuron hyperexcitability and neuroinflammation in a rat model of intervertebral foramen inflammation. J Pain. 2011 Jan;12(1):141–52. PubMed 20797917 ❐
  391. Fascinatingly, these results also demonstrate the importance of some principles of pain science, namely that interactions with the skin can be surprisingly profound for the body. And from that we can also say some nice, reasonable things about why massage therapy might be effective. There’s a lot of scientific innuendo packed into this little study!
  392. Johnson EW. The myth of skeletal muscle spasm. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 1989 Feb;68(1):1–1. PubMed 2521794 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 56710 ❐

    In this editorial from 1989, Dr. Ernst W. Johnson decries the “preposterous” widespread attribution of unexplained musculoskeletal pain to “spasm,” describing “overwhelming evidence that skeletal muscle spasm is nonexistent.” He believes that it’s a simplistic non-diagnosis with strong emotional appeal to both doctors and patients, and therefore cynically exploited by pharmaceutical companies to sell a treatment (muscle relaxants).

    Although I agree that most unexplained musculoskeletal pain has nothing to do with “spasms,” I wish I knew what “overwhelming evidence” debunks the myth: he only mentions one 1950 study that I can’t find, and a replication of it that he was involved in that I also cannot find. He describes a study of 50 people with neck and/or shoulder discomfort, none of whom “had EMG evidence of muscle activity in the area of pain or in the proximal paraspinal muscle.”

  393. There are two kinds of skeletal muscle relaxants: (1) the antispastic variety, for conditions such as cerebral palsy and multiple sclerosis; and (2) the antispasmodic variety, for musculoskeletal conditions. Antispastic agents are not discussed here, as there is no particular reason to think that they are helpful for musculoskeletal problems.
  394. I had a massage therapy client in the late 2000s who was a pharmacologist. She has a doctorate in drugs, and she was about as mainstream, credible and credentialed as they come. I asked her, “Does it seem strange to you that this claim wasn’t studied properly until just recently?” Her answer amazed me.

    “Actually, no,” she said. “It’s a bit embarrassing. Believe it or not, proper control of drug trials has really only started to become routine in the last decade. There are many examples of drugs that have only recently been studied properly … and even more that still haven’t been.”

    This is exactly analogous to the surprising lack of properly controlled trials of surgeries, especially orthopaedic surgeries. See Surgery

    .
  395. Khwaja SM, Minnerop M, Singer AJ. Comparison of ibuprofen, cyclobenzaprine or both in patients with acute cervical strain: a randomized controlled trial. Canadian Journal of Emergency Medical Care. 2010 Jan;12(1):39–44. PubMed 20078917 ❐

    A study in the Canadian Journal of Emergency Medical Care compared ibuprofen and a muscle relaxant (cyclobenzaprine or Flexeril) for patients with serious soft-tissue injury in the neck. Groups of about 20 patients received one, the other, or both. Results were statistically identical for all patients. This test showed no benefit to using or adding a muscle relaxant for acute muscle strain in the neck. The study is too small to be powerful, but it certainly shows that there’s no strong advantage to muscle relaxants in a situation where they are often assumed to be an important medication, and the results are consistent with other research results.

  396. See S, Ginzburg R. Choosing a skeletal muscle relaxant. Am Fam Physician. 2008 Aug;78(3):365–70. PubMed 18711953 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 55418 ❐

    Skeletal muscle relaxants are widely used in treating musculoskeletal conditions. However, evidence of their effectiveness consists mainly of studies with poor methodologic design. In addition, these drugs have not been proven to be superior to acetaminophen or nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs for low back pain.

    …skeletal muscle relaxants should not be the primary drug class of choice for musculoskeletal conditions.

  397. Chou R, Peterson K, Helfand M. Comparative efficacy and safety of skeletal muscle relaxants for spasticity and musculoskeletal conditions: a systematic review. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2004 Aug;28(2):140–75. PubMed 15276195 ❐ “There is insufficient evidence to determine the relative efficacy or safety of cyclobenzaprine, carisoprodol, orphenadrine, tizanidine, metaxalone, methocarbamol, and chlorzoxazone.”
  398. A few footnotes back, I mentioned a pharmacologist client. In addition to her general cynicism about the evidence-base for many common drugs, she had some really interesting comments about muscle relaxants: she believes methocarbamol is ineffective specifically at recommended dosages. Although higher dosages will indeed reduce muscle tone, the higher dosages also have much more prevalent side effects. “Somehow the drug got approved even though the low recommended dosages are virtually useless,” she told me. “Any benefit you seem to get from these drugs at normal dosages is probably a placebo and/or the result of other active ingredients, especially codeine.” 😮
  399. van Tulder MW, Touray T, Furlan AD, Solway S, Bouter LM. Muscle relaxants for non-specific low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2003;(2):CD004252. PubMed 12804507 ❐ PainSci Bibliography 52676 ❐
  400. Abdel Shaheed C, Maher CG, Williams KA, McLachlan AJ. Efficacy and tolerability of muscle relaxants for low back pain: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Pain. 2017 02;21(2):228–237. PubMed 27329976 ❐
  401. See 2008, op. cit.
  402. Flaten MA, Simonsen T, Olsen H. Drug-related information generates placebo and nocebo responses that modify the drug response. Psychosom Med. 1999;61(2):250–5. PubMed 10204979 ❐

    How much does the effect of a medication depend on what you are told about it? Quite a bit, apparently! This strange and fascinating study in Psychosomatic Medicine showed that a muscle relaxant actually increases tension when the patient is told (lied to) that it is actually a stimulant. The false information is so potent — or the drug is so weak — that its intended effect is actually reversed.

    It’s like a Jedi mind trick. These aren’t the drugs you’re looking for.

    But the reverse was not true: even when told that they were taking a muscle relaxant (and they were), subjects did not actually relax any more than people taking a placebo … and in some cases less!

    And there’s more. This study contains many odd gems, such as the bizarre fact that quite a lot more muscle relaxant was found in the blood of people who had been told that the muscle relaxant was a muscle relaxant. It appears that they literally soaked up more of the stuff from the GI tract when they believed that it was a relaxant! And yet it still didn’t actually relax them any more than a placebo.

  403. It is a myth that muscles are paralyzed by anaesthesia and that surgeons have to be extremely careful not to dislocate joints. There is still normal muscle tone with standard anasthesia, and in fact, “There is a constant battle to relax the muscles during some procedures,” explains Dr. Steven Levin [in private correspondence]. “Maybe the newer anesthetics have more curare-like effects, but if they do, they would have to intubate every patient. If the patient is breathing on their own, they have muscle tone! Sometimes, when fixing a fracture or repairing a ligament, the patient must be curarized.”

    The curare poison is the only way to truly paralyze muscle for surgery, but it’s used sparingly and specifically: it’s not part of normal anaesthesia, which only stops protective reflexes. “You have to be awfully insensitive not to know when you are exceeding tissue limits,” Dr. Levin says.

    Even a supposedly relaxed muscle shrinks about 20% when cut. The tone is mediated by the brain and spinal cord, which is not affected by anaesthesia. Some believe there is some intrinsic regulation of tone — that is, the muscle sets its own tone — but Dr. Levin directly refutes this with some pretty sound logic: “Curare works at the neuro-muscular synapse, so it is the CNS that maintains the muscle tone, including the resting muscle tone (RMT). In my many years of doing surgery, I have never cut a muscle that did not retract unless it was curare-ized (and even then there is some contraction), so the tone has to be a primitive function, maybe some of it spinal, present even in deeply anesthetized creatures.” That’s from this page, a bit hard on the eyes and heavy reading, but neat stuff.

  404. Muscle’s natural, unpowered state is rigid. It actually requires energy to relax it by (expensively) breaking the bonds between its contractile proteins. We do this constantly while alive. When we die, we run out of energy fast. Rigor mortis — muscular rigidity — starts within minutes of death, as soon as the circulating ATP [energy molecule] runs out. Muscles cannot truly “relax” after death until the proteins start to denature. Ew. For more about how muscle contraction works, see Micro Muscles and the Dance of the Sarcomeres: A mental picture of muscle knot physiology helps to explain four familiar features of muscle pain.
  405. Friedman BW, Irizarry E, Solorzano C, et al. Diazepam Is No Better Than Placebo When Added to Naproxen for Acute Low Back Pain. Ann Emerg Med. 2017 Jan. PubMed 28187918 ❐
  406. www.PaulIngraham.com [Internet]. Ingraham P. A Story of Benzodiazepine Withdrawal Gone Horribly Wrong; 2016 Sep 12 [cited 19 Nov 21]. PainSci Bibliography 52678 ❐
  407. Your respect for the power of these drugs should be made clear. State that you understand the addiction risk, and that you want to do only a short term experiment. You may encounter some resistance or even refusal. Many doctors have a blanket policy of not prescribing narcotics; others will, in perfect ignorance of the nuances of frozen shoulder etiology, be completely confident that it’s ridiculous to try to treat it with benzos. If you encounter a doctor who resists on both fronts, you’re probably just not going to get a prescription.
  408. Burton R, Sheron N. No level of alcohol consumption improves health. Lancet. 2018 09;392(10152):987–988. PubMed 30146328 ❐
  409. The toxicity of alcohol is a serious issue, and a solid reason to completely avoid chronic and binge drinking. But just because alcohol is technically unhealthy at any dose doesn’t mean it’s actually a big deal at low dosages. It’s an extremely mild poison at low dosages, and that’s a small price to pay for the potentially substantial psychological benefits. Any psych